The Syllable in Optimality Theory

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Syllable in Optimality Theory [REVIEW ] The Syllable in Optimality Theory Ed. by Caroline Féry and Ruben van de Vijver, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 2008, ix+415pp. MAFUYU KITAHARA Waseda University/MIT* Keywords: Optimality Theory (OT), opacity, syllable, cumulativity, Evolutionary Phonology (EP) 1. Introduction According to the preface, this book is based on a conference held in 1998, more than a decade ago, on the same topic as its title. The book was published first in 2003 (reissued in 2008 as paperback) including papers by contributers other than the participants of the conference. Although many updates and more extended discussion following the conference are given, some of the theoretical devices and viewpoints are already outdated in the rapidly changing world of theoretical phonology. For example, Sympathy Theory is an incomplete and flawed framework that nobody looks back on (see McCarthy (2007) for details); comparative tableaux have become popu- lar to see the relative harmony of candidates at a glance (Prince (2002)); stochastic approaches to OT helped by simulation software are flourishing (Boersma and Pater (2008)); and so on. Some of the theoretical insights about the nature of OT presented in this collection of papers are still viable, though. Therefore, the purpose of this review is not to give a thorough introduction of each paper, but to try to shed light on fundamental issues and problems in theoretical phonology through the keywords and topics raised in this book. In my view, the fore- most and outstanding issue in OT since the late 1990’s is opacity: a hidden intermediate stage of derivation plays a role in determining the output. Two articles in this book directly address this topic: McCarthy’s “Sympathy, Cu- * I would like to thank Haruka Fukazawa, John McCarthy, two anonymous reviewers, and the editors of EL for helpful comments, support, and suggestions. All errors are my own. English Linguistics 28: 1 (2011) 120–130 -120- © 2011 by the English Linguistic Society of Japan REVIEWS 121 mulativity, and the Duke-of-York Gambit” and Ito and Mester’s “On the Source of Opacity in OT: Coda Processes in German.” We will first con- centrate on these two articles to grasp the properties of the opacity problem in OT. Through the discussion of opacity, however, we will see that many theoretical assumptions about phonological representations, the architecture of the grammar, and the nature of phonological processes are shared by other articles in the volume. Those will be briefly covered in this review when it is necessary. One of the contributers to this book, Juliette Blevins published a stimulat- ing and controversial book in 2004: Evolutionary Phonology, which includes an in-depth criticism of the Optimality-Theoretic framework. Her article in this volume entitled “The Independent Nature of Phonotactic Constraints: An Alternative to Syllable-Based Approaches” shares the core argument of EP. This is an interesting contrast within a single volume: OT vs. EP. We will cover this issue in a separate section in order to see the development of major theories in a larger perspective. There are 15 papers, each comprising a chapter, in this volume. Those other than the three mentioned above are: “Overview” by the editors; “The Controversy over Geminates and Syllable Weight” by Stuart Davis; “The Syllable as a Unit of Prosodic Organization in Japanese” by Haruo Kubozono; “Prosodic Weight” by Draga Zec; “Syllables and Moras in Arabic” by Paul Kiparsky; “Semisyllables and Universal Syllabification” by Young-mee Yu Cho and Tracy Holloway King; “Onsets and Nonmoraic Syl- lables in German” by Caroline Féry; “Extrasyllabic Consonants and Onset Well-Formedness” by Antony Dubach Green; “Beyond Codas: Word and Phrase-Final Alignment” by Caroline R. Wiltshire; “Ambisyllabicity and Fricative Voicing in West Germanic Dialects” by Marc van Oostendorp; “The CiV-Generalization in Dutch” by Ruben van de Vijver; and “The Rela- tive Harmony of /s+stop/ Onsets” by Frida Morelli. As is obvious from the titles, recurrent topics up to Féry’s paper are syllable weight and semisyl- lables. From Dubach’s paper on, the topic gradually shifts toward syllable edges, syllable contact, and clusters. Those are common representational devices inherited from the derivational era. Thus, some of the papers are still concerned with OT-translation or OT-reanalysis of derivational rem- nants. However, the ALIGN family of constraints is so powerful that al- most all aspects of prosodic organization including syllables and moras are reorganized with some OT flavor. Morelli’s paper is worth-mentioning in this respect. One of the strong points of OT is its intrinsic orientation to typology. The typology of syllables involving /s+stop/ clusters successfully 122 ENGLISH LINGUISTICS, VOL. 28, NO. 1 (2011) shows the merits of the OT analysis. 2. Opacity 2.1. Revised Sympathy Theory Opacity in its original form in the derivational era was defined as in (1). (1) Opacity: Kiparsky (1973) A phonological rule ρ of the form A → B/C D is opaque if there are surface structures with any of the following characteris- tics: a. Instances of A in the environment C D b. Instances of B derived by ρ that occur in environments other than C D (1a) says that A appears even though it should have been changed to B, and (1b) says that B appears even though it should not. McCarthy (page 36 of the book under review) lays out why this is problematic for OT by using examples from Bedouin Arabic. (2) Bedouin Arabic derivation UR /katab/ /katab-at/ /kitib-at/ Gloss: ‘he wrote’ ‘she wrote’ ‘she was written’ Initial syllabifi- ka.tab ka.ta.bat ki.ti.bat cation: Syncope: I → φ/C σ kit.bat Trisyllabic V → φ/C Lσ kta.bat Deletion: Raising: a → i/C σ ki.tab kti.bat In this derivation, kti.bat is opaque with respect to Syncope, because it con- tains i in an open syllable. An interaction of stress and the above deriva- tion provides a challenge for Classic OT. The stress of the UR /katab-at/ is first assigned to the initial syllable as it is the head of the trochaic foot: (káta)bat. However, the Trisyllabic Deletion process in (2) deletes the stressed vowel resulting in a stress shift: ktíbat. Before getting into an actual tableau analysis, let us see the relevant con- straints. McCarthy proposes that, from an LL sequence, an iambic foot is achieved either by lengthening the second syllable (LH) or by deleting a mora of the first syllable (ΔL: Δ stands for a moraless semisyllable). The following GRP HARM constraint formally handles this quantitative difference, where the weight of the second syllable y is greater than the first one x. REVIEWS 123 (3) Formulation of Grouping Harmony (GRP HARM ) In an iambic foot (x ’y), |y| > |x|. (|α| ≡ weight of α in moras) The next relevant constraint is a relative faithfulness constraint which has a wide applicability over any phonological scale. The two versions in (4) provide that the input and the output are exactly the same or adjacent to each other. (4) Relative faithfulness on phonological scales a. STAY (S): Input and output have the same position on the scale S. b. STAY -ADJ (S): Input and output have the same or adjacent po- sitions on the scale S. These relative constraints are for handling Raising and Syncope pro- cesses in (2) by incorporating the duration scale: a > i > φ. This is a straightforward representation of the intrinsic duration of vowels. Thus, STAY (Dur(ation)) assigns one violation for an a → i change, and two viola- tions for an a → φ change, while STAY -ADJ (Dur) assigns one violation for an a → φ change. An antagonistic markedness constraint against these relative faithfulness constraints is called REDUCE , originally proposed by Kirchner (1996). (5) REDUCE A short vowel in an open syllable has zero duration. Assign one violation mark for each increment of duration above φ on the scale Dur. Now, the tableau in (6) illustrates the problem. The opaque candidate (6a) is the desired output, but less harmonic than the transparent candidate (6c), indicated by a leftward pointing hand. (6) Transparent candidate wins over opaque candidate /katab-at/ GRP HARM STAY -ADJ (Dur) REDUCE STAY (Dur) Opaque a. (k.tí).bat * *! ** b. (ki.tí).bat *! ** ** Transparent c. (kát).bat * * An early version of Sympathy Theory (McCarthy (1999)) can handle this situation by invoking intercandidate faithfulness relations. A sympathetic constraint SYM (pathy) measures the faithfulness between a particular sym- pathetic candidate and all the other candidates. This sympathetic candidate is chosen by a “selector” constraint, notated by the symbol . As shown 124 ENGLISH LINGUISTICS, VOL. 28, NO. 1 (2011) in (7), the opaque, but correctly winning candidate (7a) is more similar to the sympathetic candidate (7b) than (7c) to (7b). (7) Opaque candidate wins over transparent candidate via sympathy /katab-at/ GRP HARM SYM STAY - REDUCE STAY ADJ (Dur) (Dur) Opaque a. (k.tí).bat * * * ** Sympathetic b. (ki.tí).bat *! ** ** Transparent c. (kát).bat **! * * A critical revision of the theory in McCarthy’s paper has arisen from the recognition that intercandidate faithfulness is too powerful a device. The SYM constraint can directly measure the similarity on any dimension, which predicts the unattested type of opacity called feeding Duke-of-York (DY) derivations. DY derivations have the general form A → B → A, which looks like they are going forward and then backward without leaving any trace of change at the surface. McCarthy then proposes the notion of cumulativity, which indirectly mea- sures the similarity by the shared faithfulness violations. Let us take a look at the faithfulness part of the tableau in (7), recast as (8). The difference between the opaque (8a) and the transparent (8c) candidate is that the viola- tion marks for STAY (Dur) are shared by (8b) and (8a), but not by (8c) and (8a).
Recommended publications
  • Inflectional Morphology in Optimality Theory
    Inflectional Morphology in Optimality Theory A Dissertation Presented by Zheng Xu to The Graduate School in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics Stony Brook University August 2007 Copyright by Zheng Xu August 2007 Stony Brook University The Graduate School Zheng Xu We, the dissertation committee for the above candidate for the Doctor of Philosophy degree, hereby recommend acceptance of this dissertation Mark Aronoff, Professor of Linguistics Robert D. Hoberman, Professor of Linguistics Alice C. Harris, Professor of Linguistics James P. Blevins, Assistant Director of Research Research Center for English and Applied Linguistics University of Cambridge This dissertation is accepted by the Graduate School Lawrence Martin Dean of the Graduate School ii Abstract of the Dissertation Inflectional Morphology in Optimality Theory by Zheng Xu Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics Stony Brook University 2007 This dissertation proposes an inferential-realizational model of inflectional morphology (Matthews 1972, Zwicky 1985, Anderson 1992, Aronoff 1994, Stump 2001) within the framework of Optimality Theory (Prince and Smolensky 1993). Following Russell 1995, Yip 1998, Hyman 2003, MacBride 2004, I assume that the phonological information of inflectional affixes is introduced through realization constraints (RC) which associate abstract morphosyntactic or semantic feature values with phonological forms. I propose that rankings of realization constraints conform to the specificity condition, i.e. a constraint realizing a more specific morphosyntactic feature value set outranks a less specific realization constraint. I also propose that the unmarked situation in which one feature value is realized by one form (Wurzel 1989) is encoded in two universal and violable markedness constraints, *FEATURE SPLIT which bans the realization of a feature value by more than one form and *FEATURE FUSION which bans a form realizing more than one feature value.
    [Show full text]
  • An Introduction to Optimality Theory
    9781405151368_4_001.qxd 11/30/07 5:01 PM Page 1 1 An Introduction to Optimality Theory 1.1 How OT Began Around 1990, Alan Prince and Paul Smolensky began collaborating on a new theory of human language. This collaboration led in fairly short order to a book-length manuscript, Optimality Theory: Constraint Interaction in Generative Grammar. Photocopies of the manuscript were widely distributed and had a terrific impact on the field of linguistics, even though it wasn’t formally published until more than a decade later (as Prince and Smolensky 1993/2004). OT had and continues to have its greatest effect on phonology, but it has also led to important work in syntax, semantics, sociolinguistics, historical linguistics, and other areas. OT belongs on anyone’s list of the top three developments in the history of generative grammar. One of Prince and Smolensky’s goals for OT was to solve a long- standing problem in phonology. Phonological theory in the tradition of Chomsky and Halle’s (1968) The Sound Pattern of English (SPE) was based on rewrite rules. The rewrite rule A → B/C___D describes an input configuration CAD and an A → B transformation that applies to it. Rewrite rules can describe lots of phenomena, but they do a poor job of explaining how phonological systems fit together. (For a brief explanation of SPE’s main assumptions, see the boxed text at the end of this section.) To illustrate, we’ll look at some data from Yawelmani, a nearly extinct dialect of the California Penutian language Yokuts (Newman 1944).1 In this language, syllables cannot be bigger than CVC (consonant- vowel-consonant).
    [Show full text]
  • Phonetically Driven Phonology: the Role of Optimality Theory and Inductive Grounding1
    Phonetically Driven Phonology: The Role of Optimality Theory and Inductive Grounding1 Bruce P. Hayes UCLA Final version: August 1997 Written for the proceedings volume of the 1996 Milwaukee Conference on Formalism and Functionalism in Linguistics Abstract Functionalist phonetic literature has shown how the phonologies of human languages are arranged to facilitate ease of articulation and perception. The explanatory force of phonological theory is greatly increased if it can directly access these research results. There are two formal mechanisms that together can facilitate the link-up of formal to functional work. As others have noted, Optimality Theory, with its emphasis on directly incorporating principles of markedness, can serve as part of the bridge. Another mechanism is proposed here: an algorithm for inductive grounding permits the language learner to access the knowledge gained from experience in articulation and perception, and form from it the appropriate set of formal phonological constraints. 1 I would like to thank Marco Baroni, Patricia Keating, Donca Steriade, and members of talk audiences at MIT, Arizona, Tilburg, Utrecht, Cornell, UC Santa Cruz, UCLA, and the Formalism/Functionalism conference for helpful input in the preparation of this paper. Phonetically-Driven Phonology p. 2 1. Phonological Functionalism The difference between formalist and functionalist approaches in linguistics has taken different forms in different areas. For phonology, and particularly for the study of fully- productive sound patterns, the functionalist approach has traditionally been phonetic in character. For some time, work in the phonetic literature, such as Ohala (1974, 1978, 1981, 1983), Ohala and OHALA (1993), Liljencrants and Lindblom (1972), Lindblom (1983, 1990), and Westbury and Keating (1986), has argued that the sound patterns of languages are effectively arranged to facilitate ease of articulation and distinctness of contrasting forms in perception.
    [Show full text]
  • Speakers Treat Transparent and Opaque Alternation Patterns Differently — Evidence from Chinese Tone Sandhi*
    This is a printout of the final PDF file and has been approved by me, the author. Any mistakes in this printout will not be fixed by the publisher. Here is my signature and the date 06/10/2018 Speakers Treat Transparent and Opaque Alternation Patterns Differently — Evidence from Chinese Tone Sandhi* Jie Zhang 1. Introduction 1.1 Opacity The study of opacity has had a long tradition in generative phonology. Kiparsky (1973: p.79) defined two types of opacity for a phonological rule, as in (1). The first type is also known as underapplication opacity, and the opaque rule is non-surface-true; the second type is also known as overapplication opacity, and the opaque rule is non-surface-apparent (e.g., Bakovic 2007). (1) Opacity: A phonological rule P of the form A → B / C __ D is opaque if there are surface structures with any of the following characteristics: (a) Instance of A in the environment C __ D; (b) Instance of B derived by P in environments other than C __ D. In the rule-based framework, opacity can generally be derived by ordering the opaque rule before another rule that could either set up or destroy the application of the opaque rule (but see Bakovic 2007, 2011, who advocates the decoupling of opacity and rule ordering). In a surface-oriented theory like Optimality Theory (OT; Prince and Smolensky 1993/2004), however, opacity poses significant challenges. Without modification to the theory, neither underapplication nor overapplication opacity can be straightforwardly derived. A number of general solutions for opacity have been proposed within OT, including the Sympathy Theory (McCarthy 1999), Stratal OT (Kiparsky 2000, Nazarov and Pater 2017), OT with Candidate Chains (OT-CC; McCarthy 2007), and Serial Markedness Reduction (Jarosz 2014, 2016).
    [Show full text]
  • Optimality Theory
    This page intentionally left blank This page intentionally left blank Optimality Theory This is an introduction to Optimality Theory, the central idea of which is that surface forms of language reflect resolutions of conflicts be- tween competing constraints. A surface form is ‘optimal’ if it incurs the least serious violations of a set of constraints, taking into account their hierarchical ranking. Languages differ in the ranking of con- straints; and any violations must be minimal. The book does not limit its empirical scope to phonological phenomena, but also contains chapters on the learnability of OT grammars; OT’s implications for syntax; and other issues such as opacity. It also reviews in detail a selection of the considerable research output which OT has already produced. Exercises accompany chapters 1–7, and there are sections on further reading. Optimality Theory will be welcomed by any lin- guist with a basic knowledge of derivational Generative Phonology. RENÉ KAGER teaches linguistics at Utrecht University, the Netherlands. CAMBRIDGE TEXTBOOKS IN LINGUISTICS General editors: s. r. anderson, j. bresnan, b. comrie, w. dressler, c. ewen, r. huddleston, r. lass, d. lightfoot, j. lyons, p. h. matthews, r. posner, s. romaine, n. v. smith, n. vincent OPTIMALITY THEORY In this series p. h. matthews Morphology Second edition b. comrie Aspect r. m. kempson Semantic Theory t. bynon Historical Linguistics j. allwood, l.-g. anderson and ö . dahl Logic in Linguistics d. b. fry The Physics of Speech r. a. hudson Sociolinguistics Second edition a. j. elliott Child Language p. h. matthews Syntax a. radford Transformational Syntax l.
    [Show full text]
  • What Is Optimality Theory? John J
    University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Linguistics Department Faculty Publication Series Linguistics January 2007 What is Optimality Theory? John J. McCarthy University of Massachusetts, Amherst, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umass.edu/linguist_faculty_pubs Part of the Morphology Commons, Near Eastern Languages and Societies Commons, and the Phonetics and Phonology Commons Recommended Citation McCarthy, John J., "What is Optimality Theory?" (2007). Language and Linguistics Compass. 93. Retrieved from https://scholarworks.umass.edu/linguist_faculty_pubs/93 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Linguistics at ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. It has been accepted for inclusion in Linguistics Department Faculty Publication Series by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. For more information, please contact [email protected]. 1 What is Optimality Theory?1 John J. McCarthy University of Massachusetts Amherst Abstract. Optimality Theory is a general model of how grammars are structured. This article surveys the motivations for OT, its core principles, and the basics of analysis. It also addresses some frequently asked questions about this theory and offers suggestions for further reading. 1. Introduction In 1991, Alan Prince and Paul Smolensky began presenting their work on a new approach to language. By 1993, this new approach had a name — Optimality Theory — and it became known through their widely-circulated manuscript Optimality Theory: Constraint Interaction in Generative Grammar (hereafter, Prince and Smolensky (2004)). The impact of this work on the field of phonology was extensive and immediate; since 1993, it has also stimulated important research in syntax, semantics, sociolinguistics, historical linguistics, and other areas.
    [Show full text]
  • An Optimaility Theoretic Approach to Child Language Acquisition Colette Feehan University of Minnesota, [email protected]
    Linguistic Portfolios Volume 5 Article 3 2016 An Optimaility Theoretic Approach to Child Language Acquisition Colette Feehan University of Minnesota, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.stcloudstate.edu/stcloud_ling Part of the Applied Linguistics Commons Recommended Citation Feehan, Colette (2016) "An Optimaility Theoretic Approach to Child Language Acquisition," Linguistic Portfolios: Vol. 5 , Article 3. Available at: https://repository.stcloudstate.edu/stcloud_ling/vol5/iss1/3 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by theRepository at St. Cloud State. It has been accepted for inclusion in Linguistic Portfolios by an authorized editor of theRepository at St. Cloud State. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Feehan: An Optimaility Theoretic Approach to Child Language Acquisition Linguistic Portfolios – Volume 5, 2016 | 15 AN OPTIMALITY THEORETIC APPROACH TO CHILD LANGUAGE ACQUISITION COLETTE M. J. L. FEEHAN Abstract A central goal of linguistics is to develop models to account for how children acquire their native language. One study that has done this is Boersma, Escudero, and Hayes (2003). In this study, the authors developed a model using an Optimality Theoretic approach to account for the ways in which children acquire language and language specific F1 frequencies into phonetic categories. The model uses a set of Optimality Theoretic constraints whose rankings gradually change in response to the learner’s input. The way this model works, however, utilizes high- ranked discriminatory constraints in the initial state to produce movement in the Optimality Theory hierarchy. By taking this approach, it seems that the learner initially perceives incoming speech sounds as non-speech sounds and soon thereafter learns to categorize the sounds into the appropriate phonetic categories, resulting in language acquisition.
    [Show full text]
  • Optimality Theory in Morphology and Syntax Dieter Wunderlich (Berlin) Nov
    Optimality theory in morphology and syntax Dieter Wunderlich (Berlin) Nov. 2004 to appear in: Encyclopedia of Language & Linguistics – 2nd edition, Oxford: Elsevier abstract: The article gives a brief summary of work dealing with morpho-syntactic phenomena in OT, and also addresses more general problems. It highlights on questions of optimal ordering, optimal case, and optimal agreeement, including phenomena such as morphological gaps, repairs and case-split. It finally addresses the concepts of stochastic OT and bidirectional OT. key words: bidirectional OT, candidate set, case-split, constraint-ranking, faithfulness, harmonic alignment, lexical marking, optimal agreement, optimal case, optimal word, optimality theory, parallel optimization, repair, serial optimization, stochastic OT 1. Some basic issues Optimality Theory (OT) has originally been developed for dealing with phonological problems, abandoning the assumption that grammatical constraints are inviolable (Prince & Smolensky 1993/2004, McCarthy & Prince 1995). The fundamental new ideas have then soon been adopted in other grammatical domains, too, in syntax (Grimshaw 1997, Ackema & Neeleman 1998, Grimshaw & Samek-Lodovici 1998, Barbosa et al. 1998, Dekkers et al. 2000, Legendre et al. 2001) as well as in morphology (Legendre 2000, Wunderlich 2001). The main innovations of OT can be summarized as follows: (i) Grammatical constraints can conflict with each other and are violable under certain conditions (not inviolable, as assumed before). (ii) Grammatical constraints are ordered according to their respective weight (not equally ranked). (iii) Different rankings of constraints are responsible for differences between individual grammars or languages. (What was formerly conceived as grammatical parameters, is now reconstructed as constraints being differently ranked.) (iv) A construction is grammatical (not only by virtue of its own properties, or by virtue of its generative history, but rather) if it wins the competition in a candidate set, because it satisfies best the higher-ranked constraints.
    [Show full text]
  • The Routledge Handbook of Phonological Theory Current Issues
    This article was downloaded by: 10.3.98.104 On: 26 Sep 2021 Access details: subscription number Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: 5 Howick Place, London SW1P 1WG, UK The Routledge Handbook of Phonological Theory S. J. Hannahs, Anna R. K. Bosch Current issues and directions in Optimality Theory Publication details https://www.routledgehandbooks.com/doi/10.4324/9781315675428-3 Martin Krämer Published online on: 29 Dec 2017 How to cite :- Martin Krämer. 29 Dec 2017, Current issues and directions in Optimality Theory from: The Routledge Handbook of Phonological Theory Routledge Accessed on: 26 Sep 2021 https://www.routledgehandbooks.com/doi/10.4324/9781315675428-3 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR DOCUMENT Full terms and conditions of use: https://www.routledgehandbooks.com/legal-notices/terms This Document PDF may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproductions, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The publisher shall not be liable for an loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material. 3 Current issues and directions in Optimality Theory Constraints and their interaction Martin Krämer 3.1 Introduction The first manuscripts on Optimality Theory (henceforth OT; Prince & Smolensky 1993/2004) were circulated in the early nineties, which is roughly a quarter of a century ago.
    [Show full text]
  • Optimality Theory
    This page intentionally left blank This page intentionally left blank Optimality Theory This is an introduction to Optimality Theory, the central idea of which is that surface forms of language reflect resolutions of conflicts be- tween competing constraints. A surface form is ‘optimal’ if it incurs the least serious violations of a set of constraints, taking into account their hierarchical ranking. Languages differ in the ranking of con- straints; and any violations must be minimal. The book does not limit its empirical scope to phonological phenomena, but also contains chapters on the learnability of OT grammars; OT’s implications for syntax; and other issues such as opacity. It also reviews in detail a selection of the considerable research output which OT has already produced. Exercises accompany chapters 1–7, and there are sections on further reading. Optimality Theory will be welcomed by any lin- guist with a basic knowledge of derivational Generative Phonology. RENÉ KAGER teaches linguistics at Utrecht University, the Netherlands. CAMBRIDGE TEXTBOOKS IN LINGUISTICS General editors: s. r. anderson, j. bresnan, b. comrie, w. dressler, c. ewen, r. huddleston, r. lass, d. lightfoot, j. lyons, p. h. matthews, r. posner, s. romaine, n. v. smith, n. vincent OPTIMALITY THEORY In this series p. h. matthews Morphology Second edition b. comrie Aspect r. m. kempson Semantic Theory t. bynon Historical Linguistics j. allwood, l.-g. anderson and ö . dahl Logic in Linguistics d. b. fry The Physics of Speech r. a. hudson Sociolinguistics Second edition a. j. elliott Child Language p. h. matthews Syntax a. radford Transformational Syntax l.
    [Show full text]
  • Optimality and Functionality: Objections and Refutations1
    Optimality and Functionality: Objections and Refutations1 Joan Bresnan (Stanford) and Judith Aissen (UCSC) To appear in Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 20.1 The Classical Generative View The classical generative theory of knowledge of language is that a speaker’s men- tal grammar is a pure combinatorial engine, blind to typology and resistant to grammar-external forces. It arises from domain-specific innate principles of UG, whose parametric variation becomes fixed upon exposure to a given linguistic experience. In this classical epistemology, markedness hierarchies such as the animacy hierarchy cannot play a role in the individual synchronic grammar of a present-day English speaker. These hierarchies are not universal; they are exception- ridden, both across languages and even within the individual languages where their effects sometimes appear. They also have no obvious structural basis in generative representations. Hence, they are regarded as at best vague tendencies reflecting scalar properties of human perception and cognition or socio-cultural categorizations, external to the specific domain of linguistic structure. Their ap- pearance in languages typologically distant from English (e.g. Lummi, Dyirbal, Navajo) shows merely that grammar-external forces may leave their marks upon languages historically. On this view, some of the grammatical structure found in existing languages may be the conventionalized residue of external pressures on historical change, which are no longer active synchronically. But present-day speakers have
    [Show full text]
  • EPENTHESIS, DELETION and the EMERGENCE of the OPTIMAL SYLLABLE in CREOLE July, 1999
    1 Birgit Alber Ingo Plag University of Marburg University of Hannover [email protected] [email protected] EPENTHESIS, DELETION AND THE EMERGENCE OF THE OPTIMAL SYLLABLE IN CREOLE July, 1999 Abstract One of the central problems in creole studies is the nature of the processes that are involved in creolization. This paper investigates this issue with regard to the restructuring of the syllable in the genesis of one English-based creole, Sranan. In the emergence of Sranan, as in that of many other creoles, we can observe the restructuring of syllables through epenthesis and deletion of segments. These processes are, however, not uniform. For example, in some environments (e.g. certain kinds of complex onsets) deletion is preferred (cf. strong > tranga), whereas in others epenthesis is preferred (e.g. in word- final position, as in walk > waka). The paper presents a systematic analysis of the two interrelated processes in optimality theoretic terms, showing that the observed phenomena can be accounted for in a unitary fashion by the complex interaction of violable ranked constraints. Based on this analysis, we answer the question which principles govern the development of syllable structure in creolization: universal preference laws, transfer from the substrate languages or superstratal influence? We argue that all three elements are important in the creation of the creole, but each of them in a different and very specific way. The superstrate provides the segmental material which the emerging creole tries to preserve faithfully, but universal preference laws disturb faithful copying of the superstrate system. This is possible because the substrate exerts its influence imposing a particular grammar - high ranked structural constraints and low ranked faithfulness constraints - on the creole.
    [Show full text]