East Tennessee State University Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University

Electronic Theses and Dissertations Student Works

8-2007 The ffecE ts of Nicotine Conditioned Place Preference in D2 Primed Adolescent Rats: Age- Related and Gender Effects. Yoko Emily Ogawa East Tennessee State University

Follow this and additional works at: https://dc.etsu.edu/etd Part of the Chemical Actions and Uses Commons, and the Mental Disorders Commons

Recommended Citation Ogawa, Yoko Emily, "The Effects of Nicotine Conditioned Place Preference in D2 Primed Adolescent Rats: Age-Related and Gender Effects." (2007). Electronic Theses and Dissertations. Paper 2129. https://dc.etsu.edu/etd/2129

This Thesis - Open Access is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Works at Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University. It has been accepted for inclusion in Electronic Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University. For more information, please contact [email protected]. The Effects of Nicotine Conditioned Place Preference in D2 Primed Adolescent Rats:

Age-Related and Gender Effects

______

A thesis

presented to

the faculty of the Department of Psychology

East Tennessee State University

In partial fulfillment

of the requirements for the degree

Master of Arts in Psychology

______

by

Yoko E. Ogawa

August 2007

______

Russell Brown, Ph.D., Chair

Wallace Dixon, Jr., Ph.D.

Michael Woodruff, Ph.D.

Keywords: Adolescence, Early Postweanling Age, Conditioned Place Preference (CPP),

D2 Receptor Supersensitization, Elevated T-Maze (ETM), Nicotine, Quinpirole,

Schizophrenia ABSTRACT

The Effects of Nicotine Conditioned Place Preference in D2 Primed Adolescent Rats:

Age-Related and Gender Effects

by

Yoko E. Ogawa

This study investigated nicotine conditioned place preference (CPP) in two different ages

of adolescence using a rodent model of . Both 2- and 3-chambered CPP

apparatuses were used to test whether the CPP was due to an aversion to the white

chamber. Animals were neontally treated with the dopamine D2/D3 agonist, quinpirole, or

saline and raised to either early postweanling age (P 22) or adolescence (P 29). Rats were

conditioned to prefer the white chamber using nicotine. Results showed that nicotine

induced CPP and appeared to alleviate an increased stress response in D2 primed animals,

which appeared to diminish over time. Additionally, adult D2 and non-D2 primed rats were tested on the elevated T-maze. Results revealed that D2 primed rats demonstrated a

significant increase in unconditioned . This study showed that nicotine induced CPP

in D2 and non-D2 primed rats regardless of age, and D2 primed rats appear to demonstrate

an increase in stress levels that was alleviated by nicotine.

2 CONTENTS

Page

ABSTRACT ……………………………………………………………………… 2

LIST OF FIGURES ………………………………………………………………. 7

Chapter

1. INTRODUCTION ……………………………………………………………… 8

Schizophrenia ………………………………………………………………... 8

Possible Causes of Schizophrenia ……………………………………….. 10

NMDA Receptor Hypofunction Hypothesis ……………………….... 10

Cannabinoid Hypothesis ……………………………………………... 11

The Diathesis-Stress Hypothesis ……………………………………... 11

Dopamine Hyperfunction Hypothesis ………………………………... 12

Substance Abuse ……………………………………………………………… 14

Substance Abuse and Schizophrenia ……………………………………... 14

Nicotine …………………………………………………………………… 14

Nicotine Exposure in Schizophrenia ………………………………….. 15

Nicotine Exposure in Adolescence ……………………………………. 16

Sex Differences in Adolescent Nicotine Exposure ……………………. 17

Animal Models of Disease and Disorder ……………………………………… 19

Rodent Models of Schizophrenia …………………………………………. 21

Amphetamine Model or Schizophrenia ……………………………….. 21

PCP Model of Schizophrenia ………………………………………….. 22

3 Chapter Page

Neonatal Hippocampal Lesion Model of Schizophrenia ……………. 23

Dopamine D2 Receptor Priming Model of Schizophrenia ………….. 24

Conditioned Place Preference ……………………………………………….. 27

Biased and Unbiased Designs …………………………………………… 29

The Two- Versus Three-Chambered CPP ……………………………….. 30

Adolescent Animals and the CPP ……………………………………….. 32

Sex Differences and the CPP ……………………………………………. 34

Stress, the Elevated T-Maze, and the CPP ………………………………. 35

Focus of the Study …………………………………………………………... 36

Hypotheses …………………………………………………………………... 38

2. METHODS ……………………………………………………………………… 41

Experiment 1: The Two-Chambered CPP ……………………………………. 41

Subjects …………………………………………………………………... 41

Apparatus ………………………………………………………………… 41

Procedure …………………………………………………………………. 42

Neonatal Injection …………………………………………………….. 42

Early Postweanling/Adolescent Injection …………………………….. 42

Pretest …………………………………………………………………. 42

Conditioning …………………………………………………………... 43

Posttests ………………………………………………………………... 44

Experimental Design …………………………………………………... 44

Experiment 2 …………………………………………………………………... 44

4 Chapter Page

Subjects …………………………………………………………………. 44

Experiment 2a: The Three-Chambered CPP ………………………... 44

Experiment 2b: The ETM …………………………………………… 45

Apparatus ………………………………………………………………... 45

Experiment 2a ……………………………………………………….. 45

Experiment 2b ……………………………………………………….. 46

Procedure ………………………………………………………………… 46

Experiment 2a ………………………………………………………... 46

Experiment 2b ………………………………………………………... 46

Experimental Design ……………………………………………………... 47

Experiment 2a ……………………………………………………….... 47

Experiment 2b ………………………………………………………… 47

3. RESULTS ………………………………………………………………………... 48

Experiment 1 ………………………………………………………………….. 48

Early Postweanling Group ………………………………………………... 48

Adolescent Group …………………………………………………………. 49

Experiment 2a …………………………………………………………………. 50

Early Postweanling Group ……………………………………………….... 50

Adolescent Group ………………………………………………………….. 51

Experiment 2b …………………………………………………………………. 52

Baseline and Avoidance Trials …………………………………………….. 52

Escape Trial ………………………………………………………………... 53

5 Chapter Page

4. DISCUSSION …………………………………………………………………... 55

Experiment 1 ………………………………………………………………… 56

Early Postweanling Group ………………………………………………. 56

Adolescent Group ………………………………………………………... 57

Experiment 2a ………………………………………………………………... 58

Early Postweanling Group ……………………………………………….. 58

Adolescent Group ………………………………………………………... 59

Age Differences in the Two- and Three-Chambered CPP …………………... 59

Experiment 2b ………………………………………………………………... 62

Baseline and Avoidance Trials ………………………………………….... 62

Escape Trial ………………………………………………………………. 63

Conditioned and Unconditioned Fear in the ETM and CPP………………….. 64

Application to the Human Schizophrenic Population………………………… 65

Future Studies ………………………………………………………………… 66

5. REFERENCES …………………………………………………………………... 68

VITA ………………………………………………………………………………... 92

6 LIST OF FIGURES

Figure Page

1. The Two-Chambered CPP: Early Postweanling Group ………………. 49

2. The Two-Chambered CPP: Adolescent Group ……………………….. 50

3. The Three-Chambered CPP: Early Postweanling Group ...... 51

4. The Three-Chambered CPP: Adolescent Group ...... 52

5. Elevated T-maze: Baseline and Avoidance Trials …………………….. 53

6. Elevated T-Maze: Escape Trial ...... 54

7 CHAPTER 1

INTRODUCTION

Schizophrenia

Schizophrenia is a widely known chronic and severe brain disorder characterized by hallucinations, delusions, cognitive deficits, and behavioral impairments. The term schizophrenia was first coined by Eugene Bleuler in 1911, who concluded that a mental dysfunction caused schizophrenia (Wilson, 2003). After Bleuler’s initial identification of schizophrenia, it became a well-known mental disorder all over the world. Journals and articles began to widely use the term schizophrenia in 1949 (Cohen, 1949; Gottfried &

Willner, 1949; Lipton, 1949; Torkildsen, 1949).

Current statistical data indicates that approximately 2.4 million American adults, or about 1.1 % of the population age 18 and older suffer from schizophrenia (National

Institute of Mental Health, 2006). The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders, Fourth Edition [DSM-IV], has reported that schizophrenia occurs equally in both men and women (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1994). However, the estimated age of onset for men and women differs. For men, it is in the late teens to early

20s, and for women, the typical onset is in the 20s to early 30s (APA).

In order to be diagnosed, schizophrenic patients must exhibit two or more of the following characteristic symptoms for at least a month: delusions, hallucinations, disorganized speech or behavior, blunted mood, and apathy (APA, 1994). These characteristic symptoms are separated into subtypes called positive and negative symptoms. Positive symptoms are behaviors including hallucinations, delusions, and disturbed thinking, while negative symptoms are behaviors including apathy, blunted

8 mood, and poverty of speech (APA). Because of these symptoms, patients with schizophrenia are usually behaviorally impaired and experience social dysfunction (Ujike

& Morita, 2004). As a result, they usually require social support to live, such as consistent treatment with mental health providers, or living in temporary housing managed by the government or non-profit organizations.

Recent clinical studies on schizophrenic patients have revealed that the disease results in significant structural brain abnormalities (Ferrari, Kimura, Nita, & Elkis, 2006;

Hulshoff Pol et al., 2002; Nugent et al., 2007; Velakoulis et al., 2006; Yamada et al.,

2007). Through magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and computed tomography (CT), it has been shown that schizophrenics have significantly reduced cerebral and prefrontal gray matter, prefrontal white matter and enlarged lateral and third ventricles, as well as increased peripheral cerebrospinal fluid volumes compared to control groups (Ferrari et al.; Hulshoff Pol et al.). Also, findings have shown that the hippocampal tissue volumes of schizophrenic patients are significantly reduced, which is significant because the is known to play an important role in cognitive impairment observed in the disorder (Breier et al., 1992; Gothelf et al., 2000; Nugent et al.; Velakoulis et al.).

Furthermore, the volumes of the prefrontal cortical grey matter and the have been shown to be smaller compared to control subjects (Breier et al.; Premkumar, Kumari,

Corr, & Sharma, 2006). These significant structural abnormalities found in the brain are thought to be responsible for many of the exhibited behavioral symptoms of patients with schizophrenia.

9 Possible Causes of Schizophrenia

NMDA Receptor Hypofunction Hypothesis. Several hypotheses have been posited to describe the possible underlying mechanisms of schizophrenia. One hypothesis has focused on hypofunction of the glutamatergic N-methyl-D-asparate (NMDA) receptor, commonly called the NMDA receptor hypofunction hypothesis. This hypothesis originates from a clinical study using an NMDA receptor antagonist, phencyclidine

(PCP) (Luby, Cohen, Rosenbaum, Gottlieb, & Kelly, 1959). Luby et al. revealed that

PCP produces similar symptoms to those observed in schizophrenics. Similarly, Holcomb,

Lahti, Medoff, Cullen, and Tamminga (2005) conducted a clinical study on schizophrenic and non-schizophrenic patients to measure blood flow through positron emission tomography (PET). They injected a NMDA receptor antagonist, ketamine, to both groups and found that patients with schizophrenia exhibited increased blood flow in the anterior cingulate cortex when compared to non-schizophrenic subjects. NMDA receptors are reported to be involved in synaptic activity of the anterior cingulate cortex, which is responsible for cognition (Liauw, Wang, & Zhuo, 2003). Therefore, it can be concluded that glutamate transmissions of NMDA receptors in schizophrenics are more sensitive to

NMDA receptor antagonists. Furthermore, Holcomb et al. found that all of the subjects obtained higher scores in the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale when blood flow in the anterior cingulate cortex increased. In other words, a decrease in NMDA receptor function in the anterior cingulate cortex produces psychotic symptoms. Therefore,

NMDA receptor hypofunction in the anterior cingulate cortex may be involved in schizophrenia.

10 Cannabinoid Hypothesis. Two other models have been used to analyze neurochemical and behavioral mechanisms of schizophrenia. The cannabinoid hypothesis posits that schizophrenia results in part from dysfunction of the endogeneous cannabinoid system, which is caused by cannabis sativa (marijuana) use. The endogeneous cannabionoid system is involved in anxiety, fear, attention, , , and inhibitory regulatory mechanisms (Chhatwal & Ressler, 2007; Solowij & Michie, 2007).

This hypothesis is based on the clinical observation that the heavy use of marijuana produces schizophrenia-like symptoms including paranoia, depersonalization, derealization, and deficits in perceptual processes (Favrat et al., 2005; Leweke, Schneider,

Thies, Munte, & Emrich, 1999). Skosnik, Krishnan, Aydt, Kuhlenshmidt, and O’Donell

(2006) performed a clinical study on current marijuana users. They found deficits in the electroencephalogram (EEG) neural synchronization and early-stage sensory function in marijuana users. In addition, marijuana users exhibited schizotypal personality characteristics significantly more than controls (Skosnik et al.). Furthermore, De Marchi et al. (2003) revealed that the amount of endocannabinoid anandamine in the blood was higher in schizophrenic patients than the control group. This increased endocannabinoid anandamine causes dysfunction in the peripheral and the central nervous system including hallucinations and cognitive deficits (De Marchi et al.). Thus, these two clinical studies suggest that the cannabinoid system may be involved in the development of schizophrenia.

The Diathesis-Stress Hypothesis. The diathesis-stress model posits that schizophrenia is caused by stressful environments, stressful life experiences, and genetic deficits that produce increased stress sensitivity in humans (Rosenthal, 1970). The

11 hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, which releases cortisol, is activated under

stressful conditions in humans. Increased cortisol levels eventually over-activate the

mesolimbic dopamine system and cause a significant release of dopamine in the brain

(Walker & Diforio, 1997). This increased dopamine level is thought to control positive

symptoms of schizophrenia (Carlsson & Lindqvist, 1963). A clinical study on

schizophrenia and healthy patients done by Yilmaz et al. (2007) showed that the cortisol

level in schizophrenic patients is higher than in healthy individuals. Furthermore, they

found that the longer the duration of the disease, the higher the cortisol level in

schizophrenic patients (Yilmaz et al.). Interestingly, D’Souza et al. (2004) showed a

relationship between the major psychoactive component of marijuana, delta-9-

tetrahydrocannabinol (Delta-9-THC), stress, and schizophrenia. Healthy individuals

injected with Delta-9-THC exhibited increased cortisol levels and schizophrenia-like

symptoms, including cognitive dysfunctions and increased anxiety levels (D’Souza et al.).

According to these studies, both stress and marijuana use may play a role in

schizophrenia.

Dopamine Hyperfunction Hypothesis. One of the more researched hypotheses of schizophrenia is the dopamine hyperfunction hypothesis. Carlsson and Lindqvist (1963) found that schizophrenic patients suffer from increased dopamine levels, which is thought to be involved in the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. Cocaine and amphetamine are found to increase dopaminergic activity. Amphetamine and cocaine users exhibited schizophrenia-like symptoms, believed to be produced by their increased dopaminergic activity (Berger, 1981). The classic study that demonstrated the involvement of dopamine in schizophrenia was published by Randrup and Munkvad in 1967. In this study on

12 schizophrenic patients, they showed that dopamine agonists that activate the activity of

dopamine receptors produce schizophrenia symptoms. On the other hand, dopamine

antagonists that block the action of dopamine receptors have been shown to reduce

positive symptoms in schizophrenic patients (Randrup & Munkvad).

Increased dopaminergic activity in schizophrenics may be due to their

significantly increased density of dopamine D2 receptors compared to control subjects

(Pearce, Seeman, Jellinger, & Tourtellotte, 1990; Seeman, 1985). This finding indicates

increased neural activity in response to a normal amount of released dopamine (Wilson,

2003). A study in patients with schizophrenia revealed increased presynaptic activity of

dopamine neurons at D2 receptors in the striatum (Abi-Dargham et al., 2000). In addition,

through PET, Seeman and Niznik (1990) found that D2 receptor density in patients with

schizophrenia is increased in postmortem brain putamen and caudate nucleus. Therefore,

there is an increased density of D2 receptors, which helps to explain why schizophrenic

patients may have increased sensitivity to dopamine (Seeman et al., 2005). However, one

caveat to these studies is that all schizophrenics are typically medicated with

antipsychotic drugs, which all block the dopamine D2 receptor with some affinity, and are known to increase the dopamine levels and the number of dopamine D2 receptors (Risch,

1996; Seeman, 2006; Seeman, Corbett, & Van Tol, 1997; Seeman, Lee, Chau-Wong, &

Wong, 1997 as cited in Kapur, Zipursky, Jones, Remington, & Houle, 2000; Tollefson,

1996).

13 Substance Abuse

Substance Abuse and Schizophrenia

The rate of substance abuse is one of the most serious public health problems that most nations are struggling to reduce because it easily affects communities. One group of people who are at an especially high risk for abusing substances is schizophrenia patients.

It has been reported that there are higher rates of substance abuse among schizophrenics compared to the normal population (LeDuc & Mittleman, 1995). Drugs that are used in this population include cannabis, cocaine, and nicotine (Winklbaur, Ebner, Sachs, Thau,

& Fischer, 2006). Schizophrenic patients are two to five times more likely to use psychoactive drugs than non-schizophrenics (LeDuc & Mittleman). Some studies have reported that abuse of psychoactive substances in the schizophrenic population produces increased memory impairment, isolation, early life stress, and depressive symptoms

(Scheller-Gilkey, Thomas, Woolwine, & Miller, 2002; Sevy, Kay, Opler, & van Praag,

1990).

Nicotine

Nicotine is one of the substances most commonly abused by people who are diagnosed with mental disorders including depression, anxiety disorder, panic disorder, eating disorder, and schizophrenia (Anzengruber et al., 2006; Breslau, 1995; Hughes,

Hatsukami, Mitchell, & Dahlgren, 1986; Isensee, Wittchen, Stein, Hofler, & Lieb, 2003).

It is a reinforcing drug that increases dopaminergic activity in the

(Miyasato, 2001). Due to these properties, nicotine is highly addictive, and people become nicotine-dependent rapidly. Withdrawal typically produces increased anxiety

(Nerin, Beamonte, Gargallo, Jimenez-Muro, & Margueta, 2007). The DSM-IV

14 recognizes nicotine as an addictive substance that is abused by the population (APA,

1994). Small rapid doses of nicotine can be delivered to the brain within a few seconds, and nicotine binds to nicotinic acetylcholine receptors (nAChRs), which are found in both the peripheral and the central nervous system (Goodman, 1995 as cited in Uneri,

Tural, & Cakin Memic, 2006; Uneri et al.). Nicotinic acetylcholine receptors are reported to be highly concentrated in the hippocampus, where they regulate glutamate release, and also in the ventral tegmental area (VTA), where they play a role in regulating dopamine release (Fallon & Loughlin, 1995; Vizi & Kiss, 1998). The hippocampus plays a major role in cognition, and the VTA sends projections to the nucleus accumbens and plays a major role in positive reinforcement and drug addiction. In addition, Cao, Surowy, and

Puttfarcken (2005) found that nAChRs are responsible for dopamine release in the hippocampus. Furthermore, nAChRs are reported to control several neurotransmitter levels including norepinephrine, GABA, and acetylcholine in several brain areas (Clarke

& Reuben, 1996; Wilkie, Hutson, Sullivan, & Wonnacott, 1996; Yang, Criswell, &

Breese, 1996). Finally, nAChRs are found in high concentrations at the neuromuscular junction in the periphery, which indicates that they play a role in motor function

(Bernheim, Hamann, Liu, Fischer-Lougheed, & Bader, 1996). Therefore, nAChR dysfunction is thought to be involved in neurodegeneration and cognitive deficits of schizophrenia.

Nicotine Exposure in Schizophrenia. There are two groups of people who are at higher risk of being nicotine dependent. It has been reported that between 74 % and 92 % of the schizophrenic population in America is nicotine dependent (de Leon et al., 1995;

Hughes et al., 1986; O’Farrell, Connor, & Upper, 1983). The reason for the high rate of

15 nicotine abuse in this population is thought to be for self-medication purposes and to

alleviate symptoms of the disorder (Araki, Suemaru, & Gomita, 2002; Krystal et al.,

2006; Kumari & Postma, 2005; Punnoose & Belgamwar, 2006). Several studies have

supported this explanation. Nicotine has been shown to alleviate several commonly

exhibited symptoms of schizophrenia such as cognitive impairment, processing of

auditory stimuli, and anxiety (Larrison-Faucher, Matorin, & Sereno, 2004; Lyon, 1999;

Salas, Pieri, Fung, Dani, & De Biasi, 2003). Furthermore, Zhang et al. (in press) found

that positive symptoms are alleviated in smoking schizophrenics compared to non-

smoking schizophrenics, and negative symptoms are more fully alleviated the more

cigarettes patients smoke.

Nicotine Exposure in Adolescence. Another group of people who are at higher risk of abusing nicotine is adolescents. A report has shown that 42 % of middle and high school students have experimented with smoking (U.S. Department of Health and Human

Services, 1993 as cited in Wang et al., 1999). In addition, Stanton (1995) conducted a longitudinal study using 937 adolescents as subjects and found that 19.3 % of the subjects met the criteria for being nicotine dependent. Several findings have shown that smokers who develop their habits in adolescence have a lesser likelihood of quitting smoking later in life (DiFranza et al., 2000; Smith et al., 2006). These findings are supported by other studies that indicate adolescent rats have a higher sensitivity to nicotine than adult rats

(Belluzzi, Lee, Oliff, & Leslie, 2004; Rezvani & Levin, 2004). Therefore, it is believed that adolescence may be the most critical time in developing nicotine dependence.

Rodent models of adolescent nicotine exposure have revealed that nicotine exposure causes long-term changes in the central nervous system including dopaminergic

16 and catecolaminergic functioning and damage in the hippocampus, midbrain, and cerebral cortex (Abreu-Villaca, Seidler, Tate, & Slotkin, 2003; Trauth, Seidler, Ali, &

Slotkin, 2001). Trauth et al. found that nicotine infusion from P 30 to P 47.5 activated midbrain catecholaminergic pathways, resulting in increased dopamine metabolism.

However, during the post-infusion period (P 50 to P 60) midbrain catecholamine turnover decreased in male rats. On P 80, midbrain catecholaminergic pathways were activated again in males (Trauth et al.). In another study, Abreu-Villaca et al. chronically injected three different doses of nicotine (0.6, 2, and 6 mg/kg/day), and all of these doses resulted in a decreased density, number, and size of cells, meaning that nicotine produced cell death. Nicotine decreased neuron projection in the midbrain, hippocampus, and cerebral cortex, and these brain alterations were still observed 1 month after nicotine treatment.

On the other hand, 6 mg/kg/day of nicotine injection in adult rats did not produce any brain alterations (Abreu-Villaca et al.). In conclusion, these critical neuronal damages and changes caused by nicotine are thought to be key elements in causing lifetime nicotine addiction in adolescents.

Sex Difference in Adolescent Nicotine Exposure. There are some reports indicating that there may be sex differences in adolescent nicotine exposure. Piko, Wills, and Walker (in press) used 1,225 U. S. high school students as participants to analyze whether sex differences exist in cigarette smoking. The results showed that there is no significant difference between the rate of smoking in boys and girls. However, around 15 years ago, it was reported that the smoking rate was higher among boys (Johnston,

O’Malley, & Bachman, 2000 as cited in Piko et al.). Piko et al. have concluded that the rate of smoking among girls has increased since that time, and the currently equal

17 occurring rates of smoking are due to increased acceptance of female smoking in public places.

In contrast to Piko and colleagues’ study, Stanton, McClelland, Elwood, Ferry, and Silva (1996) found gender differences in the number of adolescent daily smokers (15 to 18 year-olds) in New Zealand in a longitudinal study. They found that the daily smoking rate increased from 15 % (15 year-olds) to 31 % (18 year-olds). Among girls, there was a greater number of daily smokers than boys. Two factors can possibly explain the reasons for these sex differences. Perkins, Donny, and Caggiula (1999) reported that males are more sensitive to the reinforcing effect of nicotine. Furthermore, Crisp,

Sedgwick, Halek, Joughin, and Humphrey (1999) found that teenage females have higher social anxiety from weight gain and body image. Females are shown to be more sensitive to the anxiolytic effect of nicotine than males. Therefore, Crisp et al. concluded that teenage girls smoke in order to receive the calming and anxiolytic effects of nicotine.

Sex differences have been shown in the behavioral response to nicotine in adolescent mice (Klein, Stine, Vandenbergh, Whetzel, & Kamens, 2004). Mice were given 24 hour access to either saccharin solution or one of the following amounts of freebase nicotine: 10 ug/ml, 25 ug/ml, 50 ug/ml, 75 ug/ml, 100 ug/ml, or 200 ug/ml dissolved in saccharin solution. The results showed that there is no sex difference in serum cotinine (a nicotine metabolite) levels, according to the liver weights of male and female mice. However, female adolescent mice consumed more nicotine than males. The authors reasoned that this sex difference is observed due to one of two possible reasons: either female mice are less sensitive to nicotine than males, or females metabolize nicotine more quickly than males. Therefore, Klein et al. concluded that the rate of

18 nicotine metabolism, absorption, distribution, and excretion processes among females and males may be different.

Neuroanatomically, there are sex differences in response to nicotine. Xu, Seidler,

Ali, Slikker, and Slotkin (2001) found that chronic administration of nicotine produces suppression in 5-HT activity and damage to 5-HT projection in adolescent rats. In addition, Xu, Seidler, Cousins, Slikker, and Slotkin (2002) found that female adolescent rats exhibited reduced 5-HT receptor binding due to nicotine in the cerebral cortex.

Furthermore, Viveros, Marco, and File (2006) reported that females exhibited a greater change in 5-HT receptors than males that may have contributed to 5-HT receptor dysregulation. This 5-HT receptor change in adolescent females may have contributed to sex differences in behavioral responses to nicotine.

To sum up these studies, there may be sex differences in nicotine consumption and behavioral responses to nicotine in adolescence because females may be less sensitive to nicotine, and nicotine damages 5-HT functions more in females. However, there are only a limited number of articles indicating sex differences in adolescent animals. Thus, the explanations for why there may be sex differences in behavioral response to nicotine and nicotine consumption are still unclear. Further research on gender differences in nicotine exposure is needed.

Animal Models of Disease and Disorder

The use of animal models is vital for scientific research because it allows for the possibility of finding interventions or treatments for certain diseases and also a better understanding of how they develop in humans. Animal models have been shown to be especially useful in the development of novel medications and manipulations to help treat

19 disorders. According to McKinney and Bunney (1969), in order to validate an animal model of disease, there are three criteria that need to be reached: the similarity of conditions, behavioral states, and common mechanisms. These are all critical if an animal model is said to be valid for a particular disorder. However, even if all of the criteria are not met, and only one criterion is met, the information gathered from using the “invalid” animal model can still be useful (Woodruff & Baisden, 1994). Use of animal models met with any of the criteria raised by McKinney and Bunney allows analysis mechanisms that may be important for the progression of disease. In addition, the animal models can be used to see whether treatment works for those diseases before administering them to human subjects (Wilson, 2003).

Another reason is that animal models of disease can be correlative, analogous, or homologous to the diseases or symptoms observed in humans (Gage, Bjorklund, Isacson,

& Brundin, 1985). For example, untreated animals used in a correlative animal model behave like they have been given certain drug treatments. In other words, the correlative animal model can duplicate the conditions that various different drugs would have on humans. Animals used in an analogous animal model are given lesions in the central nervous system to model certain neuropathologies that occur in the particular targeted disorder in humans. In this way, the analogous animal model can produce neurodegenerative disorders in animals such as Parkinson’s disease, Alzheimer’s disease, and schizophrenia, which normally only occur in humans. Finally, animals used in a homologous animal model exhibit all aspects of the disease as humans (Gage et al.). In other words, a homologous animal model is a complete duplication of human disease in

20 animals. Therefore, the use of animal models is very important for humans’ physical and mental health.

Several animal models of disease have been produced, especially for mental disorders such as schizophrenia, major depressive disorder, anxiety disorders, and alcohol and drug addiction. Hitzemann (2000) explained that animal models of mental disorders could be used across several different behavioral tasks that accurately test observed dysfunctions of the disease and may help to identify how mental disorders are involved in particular human behaviors. Scientists primarily use animal models of mental disorders to explore possible treatments or therapeutic approaches, and certain aspects of mental disorders can be easily modeled in rodents (Hitzemann).

Rodent Models of Schizophrenia

Amphetamine Model of Schizophrenia. Several rodent models such as the amphetamine model, PCP model and neonatal hippocampal lesion model have been used to model schizophrenia symptoms in rodents. The oldest rodent model of schizophrenia that has been prevalently used was created by the acute administration of amphetamine, which has been reported to increase dopamine release (Leyton et al., 2002; Oswald et al.,

2005). This model is designed to produce analogous symptoms to the disease by producing a hyperactive dopaminergic system. The reason this model has been questioned is that the large dose administration of amphetamine in rats creates a temporally robust dopamine release, and the dopamine system is hyperactive throughout the lifetime of schizophrenics. Castner, Vosler, and Goldman-Rakic (2005) circumvented this problem by analyzing the effects of chronic administration of amphetamine on dopamine levels and behavior over a 6-week period in monkeys. They found that a

21 chronic administration of amphetamine produces symptomology that is similar to the

positive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia, which is thought to be a result of a

decreased level of dopamine turnover in the prefrontal cortex and striatum (Castner et al.).

Furthermore, Haber et al. (1981) found that after several low doses of amphetamine were

administered, monkeys spent a longer period of time in “sit tense” postures and showed

an increased number of agonistic behaviors as well as an increased frequency of

reorienting their position.

PCP Model of Schizophrenia. Another well used rodent model of schizophrenia is based on the pharmacological action of the drug PCP. The PCP model of schizophrenia is based on chronic administration of PCP, which is an antagonist at the NMDA receptor

(glutamatergic receptor) and produces locomotor hyperactivity, cognitive deficits, prepulse inhibition (PPI) impairment, and impairment in social novelty discrimination in rodents (Harich, Gross, & Bespalov, 2007; Nabeshima et al., 2006; Rasmussen, O’Neil,

Manaye, Perry, & Tizabi, 2007; Sams-Dodd, 1998). Schizophrenics also demonstrate significant hypoactivity at the NMDA receptor. Additionally, behaviors exhibited under the influence of PCP are similar to the positive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia.

For example, rats chronically administered PCP demonstrate schizophrenic symptoms such as stereotypical behavior and social isolation (Sams-Dodd, 1996). Also, chronic

PCP administration reduces N-acetylaspartate and increases N-acetylaspartylglutamate, which are both thought to be involved in fluid balance and energy production (L. M.

Reynolds, Cochran, Morris, Pratt, & G. P. Reynolds, 2005).

Furthermore, rats treated with PCP also exhibit deficits in the central oxytocinergic system, which is thought to play an important role in social interaction

22 deficits observed in schizophrenia (Lee, Brady, Shapiro, Dorsa, & Koenig, 2005).

Because these observed symptoms from PCP administration are hypothesized to be produced due to insufficient glutamate transmission, several studies have analyzed the effect of chronically administered PCP in rodents to model behavioral deficits observed in schizophrenia using this model (Harich et al., 2007; Nabeshima et al., 2006; Wass et al., 2006). Nabeshima et al. reported that decreased glutamate transmission caused by

PCP administration resulted in cognitive impairments in mice tested in the Morris water maze (MWM). The primary criticism of this rodent model of schizophrenia is that the

PCP model requires chronic administration to be effective. The effects of chronic PCP administration are temporary in that the behavioral deficits that are consistent with schizophrenia are demonstrated only when the drug is in the bloodstream (Sams-Dodd,

1996).

Neonatal Hippocampal Lesion Model of Schizophrenia. The most well-studied rodent model of schizophrenia is the neonatal hippocampal lesion model. One of the major differences observed through functional imaging studies among schizophrenics and non-schizophrenics is that in schizophrenics there is decreased volume of the hippocampus and frontal cortex compared to healthy subjects (Breier et al., 1992; Nugent et al., 2007; Tanskanen et al., 2005; Toulopoulou et al., 2004; Velakoulis et al., 2006;

Yamasue et al., 2004). Hippocampal dysfunction presumably plays an important role in cognitive impairment and deficits in social interaction known to be present in schizophrenia (Sprick, von Wilmsdorff, Bouvier, Schulz, & Gaebel, 2006). Therefore, the purpose of this model is to try to replicate the hippocampal neuropathology that occurs in schizophrenia.

23 To create neonatal hippocampal lesions, rats are administered ablation of the ventral hippocampus (VH) on P 7. This type of lesion produces an increase in mesolimbic dopamine transmission (Wan, Giovanni, Kafka, & Corbett, 1996). Results have shown that lesioned rats exhibit a significant increase in activity after saline injection as well as after d-amphetamine treatment as adults. More importantly, they have reported that rats neonatally given VH lesions demonstrated increased sensitivity to stress compared to the group that was VH lesioned in adulthood (Lipska, Jaskiw, & Weinberger,

1993). This observed difference may be due to increased dopamine transmission (Lipska et al.). Also, Becker, Grecksch, Bernstein, Hollt, and Bogerts (1999) have found that neonatally lesioned rats exhibited less time in social interaction and increased aggressive behaviors. Furthermore, it is reported that neonatal hippocampal lesions produce memory and cognitive deficits in rats (Lipska, Aultman, Verma, Weinberger, & Moghaddam,

2002; Chambers, Moore, McEvoy, & Levin, 1996).

These behavioral changes are strengths of the model. On the other hand, there are a few weaknesses found in the model as well. In schizophrenia, observed brain abnormalities and neurochemical changes are due to alteration of connectivity; however, in this model these changes are produced by cell death. Also, behavioral changes produced by VH lesions differ depending on the age of the rodents (Wood, Lipska, &

Weinberger, 1997).

Dopamine D2 Receptor Priming Model of Schizophrenia. A final model of schizophrenia is one that has been created in our laboratory. Quinpirole is a dopamine

D2/D3 agonist when it is neonatally administered. It has been shown to produce long-term supersensitization of the dopamine D2 receptor (referred to as dopamine D2 receptor

24 priming) (Kostrzewa, 1995; R. M., Kostrzewa, J. P., Kostrzewa, Nowak, R. A. Kostrzewa,

& Brus, 2004; Brown et al., 2004a).

Several consistent features have been found between the neonatal quinpirole model of schizophrenia and data from human schizophrenia literature. First, amphetamine administration to adult rats neonatally treated with quinpirole produced a robust increase in the release of dopamine in the striatum (Nowak, Brus, & Kostrzewa,

2001). Consistently, studies using MRI and PET have shown that amphetamine administration produces a large increase in dopamine release in the striatum of schizophrenic patients (Lavalaye et al., 2001; Soares & Innis, 1999).

Second, neonatal quinpirole treatments have been shown to produce deficits in auditory sensorimotor gating using PPI (Smith, Perna, & Brown, n.d.). PPI of the startle response refers to attenuation in response to a strong stimulus (pulse) if this is preceded shortly by a weak non-startling stimulus (prepulse). PPI provides a simple operational measure of sensorimotor gating, which is often disrupted in schizophrenia (Kumari &

Sharma, 2002; Geyer et al., 2001). It has been shown that adult rats that received neonatal quinpirole treatment demonstrated PPI deficits compared to controls using different prepulse auditory intensities (73, 76, and 82 dB) and different interstimulus intervals between the prepulse and pulse (50, 100, and 150ms) (Smith et al.).

Third, neonatal quinpirole treatments have been shown to produce long-term cognitive impairment (Brown, Gass, & Kostrzewa, 2002; Brown et al., 2004a; Brown et al., 2004b; Brown, Perna, Schaefer, & Williams, 2006). It has been well-documented that severe cognitive impairments are present in schizophrenia and suggested that cognitive impairment is a core feature of the disorder (Adler et al., 1998; Adler, Freedman, Ross,

25 Olincy, & Waldo, 1999; Elvevag & Goldberg, 2000). Cognitive deficits have also been hypothesized to be associated with sensorimotor gating in patients with schizophrenia

(Geyer, Krebs-Thomson, Braff, & Swerdlow, 2001).

Fourth, chronic treatment with the atypical antipsychotic, olanzapine, (Trade name: Zyprexa) has been shown to alleviate cognitive deficits and long-term priming of the D2 receptor, which is produced by neonatal quinpirole treatment. Thacker et al.

(2006) have shown that chronic olanzapine treatment given twice daily in adulthood alleviated cognitive deficits produced by neonatal quinpirole treatment. Yawning is a behavior mediated by the D2 receptor and produced by neonatal quinpirole treatment.

Importantly, this treatment also alleviated the significant increase in yawning to control levels, essentially reversing the D2 priming effect. These data demonstrate that not only is

D2 priming likely primarily responsible for these behavioral effects, but also that antipsychotic treatments are effective in alleviating these effects. In vitro analyses showed that significant decreases in nerve growth factor (NGF) in the hippocampus that was produced by neonatal quinpirole treatment, was reversed by olanzapine, and brain tissue was not taken until after an 8-day washout (Thacker et al.).

Fifth, neonatal quinpirole treatments in rats have been shown to produce neurochemical abnormalities in adulthood that are similar to observations made in human schizophrenics. Results from this laboratory have shown that neonatal quinpirole treatment produced a 36 % decrease in choline acetyltransferase (ChAT) and significant decreases in NGF expression in the hippocampus compared to saline controls in both early postweanling and adult rats (Brown et al., 2004a; Brown et al., 2004b; Brown et al.,

2006). Consistently, studies in non-medicated schizophrenic patients have demonstrated a

26 decrease in overall NGF expression, which has been suggested to account for

neurodevelopmental abnormalities (Aloe, Iannitelli, Angelucci, Bersani, & Fiore, 2000;

Parikh, Evans, Kahn, & Mahadik, 2003).

Sixth, the polymerase chain reaction (PCR) analyses from our laboratory have

shown a significant decrease of alpha7 nicotinic receptor genetic expression in the

hippocampus of D2 primed animals. Consistently, studies have shown decreases in density of the alpha4beta2 nicotinic receptor in the hippocampus, decreases in the alpha7 nicotinic receptor subunit gene, as well as a reduction in muscarinic receptor availability in vivo in human schizophrenic patients (Durany et al., 2001; Leonard et al., 2002;

Raedler et al., 2003).

Finally, neonatal quinpirole treatment in rats produced significant decreases in the

regulator of G-protein signaling (RGS) 9, the genetic transcript that codes for the G-

protein that couples to the D2 receptor, when D2 primed animals are adults. This has also

been shown to be consistent with the microarray experiments in postmortem

schizophrenic patients (Mirnics, Middleton, Stanwood, Lewis, & Levitt, 2001). Mirnics

et al. found that RGS 4 expression was significantly decreased in the prefrontal cortex of

the schizophrenic patients. Therefore, neurochemically and behaviorally, it can be

concluded that the dopamine D2 priming receptor model is the most consistent with

symptoms of schizophrenia in humans.

Conditioned Place Preference

The conditioned place preference (CPP) is a paradigm that was developed to

measure the associative effects of drugs and distinctive environmental cues (Carr, Fibiger,

& Phillips, 1981). This relationship has been developed in rodents (Bienkowski, Kuca,

27 Piasecki, & Kostowski, 1996). The CPP behavioral apparatus usually consists of either two or three chambers. For the two-chambered CPP apparatus, usually one chamber is painted black, and the other chamber is painted white. In the three-chambered CPP apparatus, the chambers are generally painted black, white, and gray. Regardless of the apparatus used, each chamber has a different texture so that rodents can discriminate between the chambers with tactile stimuli as well as through the different color of each chamber.

Pavlov (1927) explained that involves a conditioned stimulus (CS) and an unconditioned stimulus (UCS) and defined reinforcement as strong association between the CS and UCS. The CPP is based on classical conditioning principles and involves a temporal pairing of a CS, a particular compartment, and a UCS, the drug, to measure reinforcement. In the drug-induced CPP, the conditioned response

(CR) and unconditioned response (UCR) are the amount of time the animals stay in each chamber or the amount of locomotor activity that the animals exhibit after the CS is exposed. Subjects’ CR and UCR are measured in order to understand drug reinforcement.

The CPP procedure requires repeated pairings of a UCS and CS. This repeated pairing has been shown to develop a CR in animals (Carew & Rudy, 1991; Shippenberg

& Heidbreder, 1995). After conditioning, when allowed to freely access all chambers after removing the dividers of the CPP apparatus, animals usually prefer the environment paired with the drug. This preference is the indicator of drug-induced CPP in animals.

Also, developed CS and CR relationships have been shown to block animals’ ability to become habituated to a new CS (Carew & Rudy). Although CPP was developed to test the associative effects of drugs, it has also been reported to be a useful device for

28 measuring the degree to which drugs alleviate anxiety (anxiolytic effect) (Matsuzawa,

Suzuki, & Misawa, 2000; Torrella, Badanich, Philpot, Kirstein, & Wecker, 2004).

In general, the CPP procedure usually consists of three major steps: pretest, conditioning, and posttest. The pretest is an initial test conducted on animals without any pre-drug exposure to the apparatus. During the pretest, animals are individually placed in the center of the shuttle box, and their initial compartment preference is obtained by measuring the amount of time they spend in each compartment. During the conditioning phase, one compartment is repeatedly paired with a psychoactive drug and the other compartment is paired with saline. The posttest is typically conducted the day after the final conditioning trial to record the strength of conditioning and which compartment is preferred by the animal. Therefore, the CPP is used to measure associative and reinforcing effects of positively reinforcing drugs, using classical conditioning principles.

Biased and Unbiased Designs

There are two different designs in the CPP: biased and unbiased (R. W. Brown, personal communication, February 26, 2007). The biased design allows animals to freely access any of the compartments to see which compartment they prefer when they are first exposed to the apparatus. Then, the animals are conditioned to the initially non-preferred compartment that is paired with a drug. In contrast, in the unbiased design, the animals’ initial chamber preference is not taken into account for chamber assignment, so half of the animals are randomly assigned to one compartment and the rest of the animals to another compartment. In this case, both compartments are paired with reinforcing drugs

(R. W. Brown, personal communication).

29 Even though both CPP designs have been shown to develop several psychostimulant drug-induced CPP, it has been claimed that the biased design should be used over the unbiased to measure the reinforcing effect of nicotine (Le Foll & Goldberg,

2005). Le Foll and Goldberg analyzed which nicotine dose and which CPP design (biased or unbiased) would produce nicotine-induced CPP in rats. They found that when rats are injected with 0.1 to 1.4 mg/kg of nicotine and placed in their non-preferred compartment

(biased design), nicotine induced CPP. Also, they reported nicotine induced conditioned place aversion when rats were given 2.0 mg/kg of nicotine and initially placed in their preferred compartment (Le Foll & Goldberg). A possible explanation is that the biased design is able to detect differences between before versus after conditioning because rats are initially placed in the non-preferred chamber (Schenk, Ellison, Hunt, & Amit, 1985;

Scoles & Siegel, 1986).

The Two- Versus Three-Chambered CPP

In general, most CPP used studies use either the two- or the three-chambered CPP apparatus. In both paradigms, reinforcing drugs including amphetamine, apomorphine, morphine, cocaine, and nicotine have been shown to induce CPP (Belluzzi et al., 2004;

Brielmaier, McDonald, & Smith, 2007; Le Foll & Goldberg, 2005; Nazarian, Russo,

Festa, Kraish, & Quinones-Jenab, 2004; Parker, 1992; Vastola, Douglas, Varlinskaya, &

Spear, 2002). However, Torrella et al. (2004) reported that the use of the two-chambered

CPP apparatus may produce confounding results. They concluded that obtained results from the two-chambered CPP could reflect either animal’s preference for a particular chamber or the animals’ aversion to another chamber. This aversion to a particular chamber means that they avoid the other chamber rather than prefer the chamber they are

30 in. Therefore, it is difficult to determine whether the animal’s preference for a particular chamber is due to the reinforcing effect of the drugs or the anxiolytic effect of the drugs because CPP can measure the anxiolytic effect of the drugs when it follows the biased design (Davis, Roma, Dominguez, & Riley, 2007; Torrella et al.).

Torrella et al. (2004) conducted a three-chambered CPP study with nicotine in adolescent and adult rats to see if the two-chambered CPP may be measuring the aversion to the other compartment. They reported that after conditioning sessions with nicotine, the anxiolytic effect of nicotine was observed in P 39 rats but not in P 69 rats (Torrella et al.). In addition, Vastola et al. (2002) showed that nicotine-induced CPP was observed in adolescents but not in adults when they used the two-chambered CPP apparatus and followed a similar procedure as Torrella and colleagues’ study. In contrast, it has been reported that nicotine does not induce CPP in adolescent rats when using the three- chambered CPP apparatus but does in the two-chambered CPP apparatus (A. Rauhut, personal communication, June 12, 2006). Therefore, others have reported that the two- chambered CPP apparatus may be preferred when testing nicotine-induced CPP in adolescent rats (A. Rauhut, personal communication; Torrella et al.).

In conclusion, nicotine-induced CPP is an elusive and difficult finding because it can be easily affected by age and the apparatus. Even though nicotine is a reinforcing drug, reinforcements from nicotine are not as strong as from such highly addictive drugs as morphine and cocaine. In general, the features of the two- and the three-chambered

CPP apparatuses are very similar because they contain either black/white boxes or black/gray/white boxes. However, as Torrella et al. (2004) explain, the three-chambered

CPP apparatus may be somewhat more sensitive in detecting nicotine-induced CPP than

31 the two-chambered CPP. Therefore, it can be concluded that the three-chambered CPP apparatus is a better way to assess nicotine-induced CPP.

In addition to Torrella and colleagues’ claim, there are a few additional advantages reported to using the three-chambered CPP apparatus rather than the two- chambered CPP apparatus. First, Tzschentke (1998) reported that having a third compartment provides a neutral environment that can serve as a starting point. As a result, the three-chamber CPP paradigm allows the subject to decide which chamber it prefers

(Torrella et al., 2004).

Secondly, Parker (1992) performed a study using the three-chambered CPP paradigm in rats to see if outcomes of the CPP are due to novelty interference with habituation. Several psychoactive drugs were paired (amphetamine, apomorphine, and morphine) to one compartment, and saline was paired to the other compartment. The results showed that rats preferred a compartment paired with psychoactive drugs more than the other compartments. Interestingly, the results also showed that rats repeatedly preferred a novel chamber more than a compartment paired with saline (Parker).

Therefore, it appears that the three-chambered CPP has the advantage of measuring outcomes in the CPP on the drug-free test that the two-chambered CPP apparatus presumably cannot measure (Tzschentke, 1998). However, there is not much literature that discusses the two- versus three-chambered CPP issue, so further research on this topic is required.

Adolescent Animals and the CPP

Several studies have shown that nicotine only induced CPP consistently in adolescent animals but not in adults. Brielmaier et al. (2007) used the two-chambered

32 CPP paradigm and biased design in early adolescent and adult rats. They found that acute injection of nicotine induced CPP in early adolescent animals but not in adult rats.

Furthermore, Belluzzi et al. (2004) have shown that early adolescent rats (P 28) showed nicotine-induced CPP when they are given acute nicotine administration of 0.5 mg/kg but not late adolescent (P 38) or adult rats (P 90). In addition, when late adolescent and adult rats were given nicotine repeatedly, late adolescent rats exhibited more tolerance and to nicotine than adult rats. Both studies concluded that adolescents are more sensitive to the reinforcing effect of nicotine, especially early adolescent animals

(Belluzzi et al.; Brielmaier et al.)

Furthermore, Vastola et al. (2002) found that even relatively lower doses of nicotine administration induced CPP in adolescent rats but not in adult rats when they used the two-chambered CPP apparatus and biased design. They hypothesized that this is because adolescents are more likely to be nicotine dependent and sensitive to nicotine than adults, so adolescent rats showed nicotine-induced CPP even with relatively lower doses of nicotine (0.6 mg/kg) (Vastola et al.). Furthermore, neuroanatomically, it has been reported that adolescent and adult rats differ. Kalsbeek, Voorn, Buijs, Pool, and

Uylings (1988) found that rats’ dopamine containing fibers keep developing until rats are

60 days old. These results appear to show that only adolescent rats may demonstrate nicotine-induced CPP.

To sum up these studies, it can be concluded that the biased design should be used to measure nicotine-induced CPP and the dose should be in the range of 0.1 to 1.4 mg/kg

(Belluzzi et al., 2004; Brielmaier et al., 2007; Le Foll & Goldberg, 2005; Vastola et al.,

2002). Also, it is important to note that adolescents should be used to measure nicotine-

33 induced CPP because adults are less likely to exhibit nicotine-induced CPP. In other words, nicotine-induced CPP appears to be very age specific (Belluzzi et al.; Vastola et al.).

Sex Differences and the CPP

Several studies have reported that there may be sex differences in psychostimulant-induced CPP (Balda, Anderson, & Itzhak, 2006; Nazarian, Russo, Festa,

Kraish, & Quinones-Jenab, 2004; Russo et al., 2003a; Russo et al, 2003b). Balda et al. showed that even though cocaine-induced CPP appears in both female and male adult rats, females exhibited cocaine-induced CPP to a stronger degree. Females preferred the compartment paired with cocaine more than males. This is thought to be because females are more sensitive to cocaine due to hormonal effects (Balda et al.).

Other studies have supported this hypothesis, and also that male rats take more sessions to show cocaine-induced CPP (Russo et al. 2003a; Russo et al. 2003b). To determine whether this result is due to hormonal differences, Russo et al. (2003a) gonadectomized both male and female rats. This gonadectomy procedure did not influence cocaine-induced CPP in males or females. However, ovariectomized female rats spent less time in the cocaine paired chamber than non-ovariectomized female rats, although cocaine-induced CPP was observed in both groups of female rats. Russo et al.

(2003a) concluded that these sex differences in cocaine-induced CPP may be due to decreased levels of dopamine and 5-HT produced by ovariectomy. Furthermore, Nazarian et al. (2004) found that cocaine-induced CPP was observed in female rats and reasoned that this sex difference in cocaine-induced CPP resulted from different sensitivities to cocaine in the dopamine D1 receptor. In conclusion, these studies suggest that sex

34 differences in D2 primed adolescent rats may be observed in nicotine-induced CPP.

However, there are not any reports of sex differences observed in nicotine-induced CPP

in current published literature. If sex differences are observed in nicotine-induced CPP, it

would mean that nicotine-induced CPP may be mediated by hormonal effects. Also,

dopamine and 5-HT may be involved in nicotine-induced CPP differently in males and

females.

Stress, the Elevated T-Maze, and the CPP

Some studies have reported that in patients with schizophrenia, dopamine and

stress levels are increased, as stress and dopamine levels are reported to be positively

correlated (Cabib & Puglisi-Allegra, 1996; Carlsson & Lindqvist, 1963; Finlay &

Zigmond, 1997; Yilmaz et al., 2007). In addition, Rodgers, Nikulina, and Cole (1994)

reported that when mice were treated with 0.5 mg/kg of a D2 receptor agonist, they

experienced increased stress and anxiety responses. On the other hand, treatment with

2.5-20.0 mg/kg of a D2 receptor antagonist decreased those responses (Rodgers et al.).

Therefore, there is a possibility that the dopamine D2 receptor priming model of schizophrenia, which is produced by neonatal quinpirole treatment, increases stress and anxiety levels.

The increased stress and anxiety levels could possibly influence the result of the

CPP. When rats are experiencing increased stress and anxiety levels, their preference for the darker colored environment increases because rats are nocturnal. Because the CPP apparatus contains black and white chambers, increased stress and anxiety levels could drastically differentiate the results among D2 and non-D2 primed animals. Therefore, it is

35 important to determine whether D2 primed animals have higher stress and anxiety responses than non-D2 primed animals.

The elevated T-maze (ETM) was developed to test the animal model of anxiety

and fear (Graeff, Viana, & Tomaz, 1993). It tests fear and stress responses in four

consecutive trials: baseline, avoidance 1, avoidance 2, and escape (Trivedi & Coover,

2004). Because this experiment is conducted in 1 day, the ETM measures the acute stress

and fear responses of rodents, rather than chronic stress and fear levels. The ETM has

three arms: one enclosed arm and two open arms (Zangrossi & Graeff, 1997). The open

arms are located facing each other, and the enclosed arm is located perpendicular to the

two open arms (Graeff, Netto, & Zangrossi, 1998; Zangrossi & Graeff). The ETM is

capable of measuring two different types of fear or anxiety in animals: conditioned and

unconditioned fear (Graeff et al., 1998; Viana, Tomaz, & Graeff, 1994). Conditioned fear

is determined in the first three of the four consecutive trials based on how long the animal

takes to leave the enclosed arm to enter either of the two open arms (Zangrossi & Graeff).

On the fourth trial, unconditioned fear is measured. It is determined by how long the

animal takes to leave the open arm to enter the enclosed arm (Zangrossi & Graeff). In

conclusion, because the ETM is capable of measuring both conditioned and

unconditioned fear, the ETM results of D2 and non-D2 primed animals can determine whether results obtained in the CPP are influenced by D2 priming, or not.

Focus of the Study

Nicotine is a widely abused psychostimulant drug, especially in adolescents. Also,

clinical studies have shown that schizophrenic patients are two to five times more likely

to abuse psychoactive drugs, especially nicotine (de Leon et al., 1995; Hughes et al.,

36 1986; LeDuc & Mittleman, 1995; O’Farrell et al., 1983). Therefore, this study will use the D2 receptor priming model of schizophrenia to analyze nicotine-induced CPP in both early postweanling (P 22-P 40) and adolescent (P 29-P 47) rats.

Adolescents demonstrate a significantly different response to nicotine than adults on several behavioral and neurochemical measures. A report indicated that around 42 % of teenagers have some experience with smoking cigarettes (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1991 as cited in Wang et al., 1999). The initiation of smoking in adolescence leads to a lesser likelihood of termination of smoking in adulthood (DiFranza et al., 2000; Smith et al., 2006). Furthermore, rodent studies have shown that adolescents have higher sensitivity to nicotine than adults (Belluzzi et al., 2004; Brielmaier et al.,

2007; Rezvani & Levin, 2004). Therefore, it is not surprising that there are age differences in the neurochemical and behavioral responses to nicotine in adolescence compared to adulthood (Abreu-Villaca et al., 2003; Belluzzi et al.; Brielmaier et al.;

Trauth et al., 2001). However, there is no defined mechanism as to why this age difference in drug effects exists.

The CPP is an excellent measure of the associative effects of drugs and environmental context (Bienkowski et al., 1996; Carr et al., 1981). There are several advantages to using CPP to study drug addiction. First, it is easily applicable to situations by measuring how environmental cues can trigger relapse. Second, it measures behavioral outcomes when the drug is not present (Tzschentke, 1998). Third, CPP has been reported to be effective for testing the anxiolytic or anxiogenic effects of drugs

(Torrella et al., 2004). Finally, the CPP may be especially effective at testing the effects of nicotine in adolescent rats. Findings have shown that nicotine-induced CPP can

37 generally be produced in adolescent rats, but this has not been the case in adults

(Brielmaier et al., 2007; Vastola et al., 2002). Studies have shown that early postweanling

rats (P 28) and adolescent rats (P 39) demonstrated nicotine-induced CPP, whereas adult

rats (P 69 or P 90) did not demonstrate nicotine-induced CPP (Belluzzi et al., 2004;

Torrella et al.).

Rodgers et al. (1994) found that quinpirole treatment produced increased stress

and anxiety responses in mice on the elevated plus-maze test. Therefore, in the present

study, rats neonatally treated with quinpirole may demonstrate a significant increase in

stress levels on the CPP tests. In order to confirm this hypothesis, an ETM behavioral

task was added as Experiment 2b to the behavioral tasks used in the present study. The

ETM is a task that has been used to test behavioral stress in animals (Graeff et al., 1993).

In Experiment 2b, a separate group of adult rats at P 60, which were neonatally treated

with either quinpirole or saline, was tested on the ETM.

The aim of this study was to investigate the interaction of nicotine with different

ages of adolescent D2 primed rats using both the two- and three-chambered CPP

apparatuses. Essentially, the experimental design for these CPP experiments was the

same, with the only difference between the two being the different apparatuses utilized.

Hypotheses

Hypothesis 1. Neonatal quinpirole treatment (D2 priming) will produce higher

stress and anxiety levels in rats; therefore, D2 primed animals will prefer the darker colored compartment. This hypothesis is based on the following findings. First, a microdialysis study in rats has shown that when mild stressors were present, dopamine and norepinephrine levels in the medial frontal cortex significantly increased (Cenci,

38 Kalen, Mandel, & Bjorklund, 1992). In addition, an elevated plus-maze study by Rodgers et al. (1994) reported that mice injected with 0.5 mg/kg of quinpirole demonstrated an increase in freezing behavior and showed less exploratory behaviors. Furthermore, rats are nocturnal, so they naturally prefer a darker colored environment, especially when they are stressed and have a high anxiety level.

Hypothesis 2. Animals that are conditioned with nicotine will show nicotine- induced CPP regardless of neonatal treatment. This is based on findings that have shown nicotine induced CPP in adolescent rats (Janhunen, Linnervuo, Svensk, & Ahtee, 2005;

Le Foll & Goldberg, 2005).

Hypothesis 3. Animals neonatally treated with quinpirole and conditioned with nicotine will produce a higher preference for the white compartment compared to animals neonatally treated with saline and conditioned with nicotine. The basis for this hypothesis is that neonatal quinpirole treatment produces a rodent model of schizophrenia, and approximately 85 % of the schizophrenic population in America is nicotine dependent (de

Leon et al., 1995; Hughes et al., 1986; O’Farrell et al., 1983). Also, D2 priming increases dopamine activity that reinforces the effects of nicotine.

Hypothesis 4. Nicotine will enhance CPP in younger animals due to possible withdrawal effects from chronic quinpirole treatments. This hypothesis is based on observations from past studies. Adult rats tend to become sick after nicotine injections and take longer to build tolerance to nicotine than younger animals. The possible reason for this age difference may be because adult rats experience withdrawal effects from chronic quinpirole treatments more severely than younger animals.

39 Hypothesis 5. Animals neonatally treated with quinpirole will demonstrate a significant increase in behavioral stress on the ETM. This increase in stress will be manifested as a significant increase in freezing behavior, which is the species-specific response for fear in rats, on the escape trial of the ETM. This hypothesis is based on the finding showing that mice treated with quinpirole demonstrated increased stress response on the elevated plus-maze (Rodgers et al., 1994).

40 CHAPTER 2

METHODS

Experiment 1: The Two-Chambered CPP

Subjects

In this study, two age groups were used: early postweanling (P 22-P 40) and adolescent (P 29-P 47). Female and male Sprague-Dawley (SD) rats were obtained from

Harlan Laboratories, Inc (Indianapolis, ID) and mated in order to produce offspring that were used as subjects. A total of 67 SD male and female rats (early postweanling group n

= 36; adolescent group n = 31) were used for Experiment 1. After neonatal drug treatment was complete, all animals were weaned at P 21 and assigned to their respective groups. All of the animals were assigned to either saline or nicotine (experimental group:

Male Saline Nicotine, Male Quinpirole Nicotine, Female Saline Nicotine, and Female

Quinpirole Nicotine; control group: Male Quinpirole Saline, Male Saline Saline, Female

Quinpirole Saline, and Female Saline Saline). The animals were socially housed in cages in groups of two to three in a climate-controlled vivarium with food and water available ad libitum with a 12 hour on/off light/dark cycle. All procedures are approved by the

University Committee on Animal Care (UCAC) at East Tennessee State University

(ETSU) and the vivarium is fully accredited by the Association for the Assessment and

Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care (AAALAC).

Apparatus

The two-chambered CPP paradigm was constructed of plywood (30 inch long x

18 inch wide x 20 inch high) and consisted of a black colored chamber and a white colored chamber. These chambers were separated by a removable painted wooden

41 divider. The walls of the chambers were painted with a flat finish. The bottom of the

white colored chamber was painted flat white and covered with studded plastic. The

bottom of the black colored chamber was painted with a textured black paint. The

different textures were used so that the animals could better discriminate between the

black and white chambers. The apparatus was located in a room surrounded with several

electric fans, which helped to eliminate background noise. A video camera was located

on the ceiling approximately 10 feet above the CPP chamber for recording purposes. The

overhead lights were turned on during the conditioning sessions and testing trials.

Procedure

Neonatal Injection. All of the animals were injected intraperitoneally (i.p.) with either quinpirole HCl (1 mg/kg) or saline (0.9 %, 1 mg/kg) from P 1 to P 21. The animals were injected at approximately 9:00 AM every day.

Early postweanling/Adolescent Injection. On pretest, posttest1, and posttest 2 all

of the animals were injected i.p. with saline only (0.9 %, 1 mg/kg). During the mornings

of conditioning days (approximately 9:00 AM daily), all of the animals were i.p. injected

with the drug they were assigned, which was either nicotine (0.8 mg/kg free base) or

saline (0.9 %, 1 mg/kg), 10 minutes prior to being placed into the CPP apparatus. In the

afternoons (approximately 1:00 PM daily), all of the animals received only saline (0.9 %,

1 mg/kg), 10 minutes prior to being placed into the CPP apparatus.

Pretest. Pretests were given at two different ages. For the early postweanling rats, the pretest was administered 1 day after weaning at P 22. In the adolescent rats, the pretest was given at P 29. All animals were given a pretest trial to determine initial preference. The dividers between each chamber were removed before the pretest, in order

42 to allow the animals to have free access to all chambers in the apparatus. Each animal was released on the dividing line of the black and white chambers in the two-chambered

CPP apparatus. Animals were left in the box for 5 minutes (300 seconds) and the video camera recorded all animals’ movements on videotape, which was later scored by the experimenter. For each subject, the amount of time spent in each chamber was recorded.

Additionally, the number of shuttles made by each animal, which were defined as the movement from one chamber to the other, was also recorded.

Conditioning. The early postweanling rats were conditioned from P 23 to P 39, while adolescent rats were conditioned from P 30 to P 46. Conditioning sessions began the day after the pretest. For all conditioning trials, the removable wooden dividers were placed back into each chamber. All animals were given two conditioning sessions

(morning and afternoon) per day. In the morning session (started at approximately 9:00

AM daily), the animals injected with nicotine were placed in the white compartment 10 minutes after the injection and spent 300 seconds in the chamber. Animals injected with saline were placed into the black chamber. In the afternoon session (started at approximately 1:00 PM daily), animals that were injected with saline in the morning and were also injected with saline in the afternoon session were placed in the white chamber.

Rats that were injected with nicotine in the morning and saline in the afternoon were placed in the black chamber. After each conditioning session, all of the animals were placed back in their home cage. All animals were given a total of 16 days of conditioning.

There was a posttest given after the first 8 conditioning days, and another posttest given after 16 days of conditioning. This procedure is based on that of Torrella et al. (2004), which showed that this procedure produced nicotine-induced CPP in adolescent rats.

43 Posttests. There were two posttests; posttest 1 was administered in early postweanling rats at P 31, and posttest 2 at P 40. Adolescent rats were given posttest 1 at

P 38 and posttest 2 at P 47. On each posttest, all of the animals were given saline. The dividers between the chambers were removed in order to let the animals freely explore the CPP apparatus. The animals were released into the center of the CPP box and left in the box for 300 seconds. The video camera recorded movement, and the experimenter later recorded the amount of time each rat spent in each chamber as well as the number of shuttles. After the sessions were administered and recorded, the animals were placed back in the cages and returned to the animal colony.

Experimental Design. An initial 2 (age: early postweanling or adolescence) x 2

(neonatal drug treatment: quinpirole or saline) x 2 (early postweanling/adolescent drug treatment: nicotine or saline) x 2 (repeated measure: posttest 1 or 2) four-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed and a robust significant age main effect was revealed

F(1, 59) = 45.88, p < .001. Based on this significant age difference, it was decided to analyze the two age groups separately. For each age, A 2 (neonatal drug treatment) x 2

(early postweanling/adolescent drug treatment) x 2 (repeated measure) three-way

ANOVA was used. The dependent measure, which was calculated in seconds, was the amount of time spent in each chamber. It was expressed as the amount of time spent in the white chamber subtracted from the amount of time spent in the black chamber.

Experiment 2

Subjects

Experiment 2a: The Three-Chambered CPP. Like in Experiment 1, two age groups were used: early postweanling and adolescent. A total of 62 SD male and female

44 rats (early postweanling group n = 36; adolescent group n = 26) were used. All of the animals were assigned to either saline or nicotine (experimental group: Male Saline

Nicotine, Male Quinpirole Nicotine, Female Saline Nicotine, and Female Quinpirole

Nicotine; control group: Male Quinpirole Saline, Male Saline Saline, Female Quinpirole

Saline, and Female Saline Saline). After neonatal drug treatment was complete, all animals were weaned at P 21 and assigned to their respective groups.

Experiment 2b: The ETM. In this study, adult rats (P 60) were used as subjects. A total of 38 SD adult male and female rats (Female Quinpirole Saline group n = 7; Female

Saline Saline group n = 10; Male Quinpirole Saline group n = 9; Male Saline Saline group n = 12) were used. All of the animals were assigned to saline only (experimental group: Female Quinpirole Saline and Male Quinpirole Saline; control group: Male Saline

Saline Female Saline Saline). After neonatal drug treatment was complete, all animals were weaned at P 21 and assigned to their respective groups.

The animals were socially housed in cages in groups of two to three in a climate- controlled vivarium with food and water available ad libitum with a 12 hour on/off light/dark cycle. All procedures are approved by the UCAC at ETSU and the vivarium is fully accredited by the AAALAC.

Apparatus

Experiment 2a. The three-chambered CPP apparatus was constructed of plywood

(30 inch long x 12 inch wide x 24 inch high) and consisted of a black colored chamber on one end, a white colored chamber on the other end, and a gray colored chamber in between the black and white chambers. All of the chambers were separated by a removable painted wooden divider. The walls of the black, white, and gray chambers and

45 the bottom of the gray chamber were painted with a flat finish. The bottom of the white colored chamber was painted a glossy white, and the bottom of the black colored chamber was painted with a textured black paint to help the animals discriminate between the black and white chambers. The three-chambered CPP apparatus was in the same room as the two-chambered CPP apparatus and was also surrounded by electric fans that helped to eliminate background noise. A video camera was located on the ceiling approximately 10 feet above the three-chambered CPP paradigm. The overhead lights were turned on during the conditioning sessions and testing trials.

Experiment 2b. The ETM was constructed of wood and Plexiglas. It consisted of three arms (20 inch long x 5 inch wide): one enclosed arm and two open arms, which were painted black. One arm (enclosed arm) was enclosed by a 16 inch tall piece of

Plexiglas. The other two arms (open arms) were enclosed by 0.4 inch tall pieces of

Plexiglas for fall prevention. The whole ETM was elevated 20 inches above the floor. An experimenter was present to record the results. The overhead lights were turned off during the testing trials, and the only illumination was a dim light (75 watts).

Procedure

Experiment 2a. All of the early postweanling and adolescent animals used in

Experiment 2a were given neonatal injections, early postweanling/adolescent injections, conditioning, pretest, and posttests, identical to the procedures of Experiment 1.

Experiment 2b. There were a total of four consecutive ETM trials, and all testing was administered at P 60. In the first three trials, each animal was released facing towards the center of the two open arms at the end of the enclosed arm. Each animal was given

300 seconds, and the experimenter recorded the amount of time the animal took to fully

46 exit the enclosed arm with all four paws. If an animal did not leave the enclosed arm within 300 seconds, it was removed from the ETM by the experimenter, and the session was ended. On the fourth trial, the animal was released at the end of the right open arm facing towards the center of the maze and given 300 seconds. The experimenter recorded the amount of time the animals took to fully exit the open arm with all four paws. After

300 seconds, the session was ended. After the sessions were administered and recorded, the animals were placed back in the cages and returned to the animal colony. This procedure is based on that of Trivedi and Coover (2004).

Experimental Design

Experiment 2a. Experimental design used in the three-chambered CPP study was identical to Experiment 1.

Experiment 2b. A 2 (gender: female or male) x 2 (neonatal drug treatment: quinpirole or saline) x 4 (repeated measure: baseline, avoidance 1, 2, and escape) three- way ANOVA was used. The dependent measure is the amount of time animals take to fully leave either the open arm or the enclosed arm, which is expressed in seconds.

47 CHAPTER 3

RESULTS

Experiment 1

Early Postweanling Group

The results for the early postweanling group are presented in Figure 1. A 2 x 2 x 2

three-way ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of neonatal drug treatment F(1, 31)

= 4.53, p < .04 and early postweanling drug treatment F(1, 31) = 9.05, p < .005. Animals

neonatally treated with saline and conditioned with nicotine (S-N Group) visited the

white compartment more often than controls at posttest 1, as a lower mean on the figure

indicates a stronger preference for the white chamber than the other groups. At posttest 2,

both S-N and Q-N Groups visited the white chamber more often compared to controls (S-

S and Q-S Groups). In addition, at both posttests, animals neonatally treated with

quinpirole and conditioned with saline (Q-S Group) demonstrated a strong preference for

the black chamber compared to controls. Interestingly, animals neonatally treated with

quinpirole and conditioned with nicotine (Q-N Group) visited the white chamber more at

both posttests compared to Q-S Group, although Q-N Group did not visit the white

chamber as much as S-N Group at posttest 1. Thus, it appears that D2 priming produced a

black chamber preference, and nicotine may have alleviated the black compartment

preference at both posttests.

48 Figure 1. The Two-Chambered CPP: Early Postweanling Group.

** indicates greater than all other groups, p < .05. * indicates significant difference, p <

.05.

Adolescent Group

The results for the adolescent group are presented in Figure 2. A 2 x 2 x 2 three-

way ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of adolescent drug treatment F(1, 27) =

10.55, p < .001, and significant interaction of Adolescent Drug Treatment x Posttest Day

F(1, 27) = 5.26, p < .03. Unlike the results from the early postweanling groups in the

two-chambered CPP study, there was no significant difference between Q-N and S-N

Groups at posttest 1. Both Q-N and S-N Groups visited the white compartment more

often compared to Q-S Group and S-S Group, which was the group neonatally treated

and conditioned with saline. Also, at posttest 2, there were no significant differences

between the groups, and all groups demonstrated a significant preference for the white

chamber. Thus, adolescent Q-S Group did not demonstrate a preference for the black

49 chamber after an additional 8 days of conditioning, as they did at a younger age in the

two-chambered CPP study.

Figure 2. The Two-Chambered CPP: Adolescent Group.

* indicates significant difference, p < .05.

Experiment 2a

Early Postweanling Group

The results for the early postweanling group are presented in Figure 3. A 2 x 2 x 2

three-way ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of early postweanling drug

treatment F(1, 32) = 9.1, p <.005, day of testing F(1, 32) = 7.75, p <.009, and significant

two-way interactions of Neonatal Drug Treatment x Day of Testing F(1, 32) = 3.95, p

<.05 and Early Postweanling Drug Treatment x Day of Testing F(1, 32) = 7.30, p <.01.

Although Q-N Group did not equal S-N Group, both Q-N and S-N Groups visited the white chamber more often compared to Q-S and S-S Groups at posttest 1. However, at posttest 2, this nicotine-induced CPP in S-N and Q-N Groups diminished. In fact, all of animals showed a preference for the black compartment at posttest 2. Furthermore, at

50 both posttests, Q-S Group showed a preference for the black chamber, as younger

animals had shown in Experiment 1, and nicotine appears to have alleviated this

preference.

Figure 3. The Three-Chambered CPP: Early Postweanling Group.

** indicates greater than all other groups, p < .05. * indicates significant difference, p <

.05.

Adolescent Group

The results for the adolescent group are presented in Figure 4. A 2 x 2 x 2 three-

way ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of neonatal drug treatment F(1, 26) =

4.36, p < .05 and adolescent drug treatment F(1, 26) = 6.63, p < .02. Similar to the results

observed in early postweanling groups in the three-chambered CPP study, Q-N and S-N

Groups visited the white chamber more than Q-S and S-S Groups at posttest 1. In

contrast, at posttest 2, this effect diminished, and there were no significant differences

across groups. In addition, the black compartment preference observed in Q-S Group at

51 posttest 1 was not persistent at posttest 2. It appears that nicotine alleviation of the black

compartment preference in D2 primed animals was not persistent to posttest 2.

Figure 4. The Three-Chambered CPP: Adolescent Group.

** indicates greater than all other groups, p < .05. * indicates significant difference, p

< .05.

Experiment 2b

Baseline and Avoidance Trials

The results of baseline and avoidance trials in both D2 and non-D2 primed groups

are presented in Figure 5. A 2 x 2 x 4 three-way ANOVA revealed a significant main

effect of gender F(1, 34) = 5.60, p < .03, but no other main effects or interactions were

statistically significant. Female animals took significantly less time to enter one of the

open arms than the male animals in the first three trials. There was no effect of neonatal

drug treatment on baseline and avoidance trials. This result demonstrated the lack of

statistical effect on conditioned fear between D2 and non-D2 primed animals.

52 Figure 5. Elevated T-maze: Baseline and Avoidance Trials.

Escape Trial

The results of escape trials in both D2 and non-D2 primed groups are presented in

Figure 6. A 2 x 2 ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of neonatal drug treatment,

but no significant main effect of sex or significant interaction of Neonatal Drug

Treatment x Sex. D2 primed animals remained in the open arm for a longer period of time

than non-D2 primed rats. This result appears to indicate that neonatal quinpirole treatment produced significant increases in stress and possibly unconditioned fear response in D2

primed rats.

53 Figure 6. Elevated T-Maze: Escape Trial.

* indicates significant difference, p < .05.

54 CHAPTER 4

DISCUSSION

The aim of this study was to investigate how nicotine-induced CPP develops in both D2 and non-D2 primed early postweanling and adolescent animals using both the

two- and three-chambered CPP paradigms. Overall, consistent results were observed in

early postweanling and adolescent animals at posttest 1 (P 31 and P 38) in both studies.

First, nicotine induced CPP in both early postweanling and adolescent rats neonatally

treated with either saline or quinpirole. Also, neonatal quinpirole treatment produced an

aversion to the white compartment and a preference for the black chamber in D2 primed animals. Third, nicotine appeared to alleviate this preference for the black compartment.

In addition to the two- and three-chambered CPP studies, D2 and non-D2 primed rats

were behaviorally tested on the ETM to investigate whether D2 primed rats demonstrated

an increased stress response. Indeed, results showed that D2 primed animals exhibited

significantly elevated fear compared to non-D2 primed animals on the ETM. Based on

this result, D2 priming appears to elevate stress and fear levels. This is especially true of unconditioned fear, but not conditioned fear.

There were significant age differences observed in both the two- and three-

chambered CPP studies. At posttest 1 early postweanling S-N Groups visited the white

compartment more than the adolescent S-N Groups. This effect may stem from different

dopamine D2 densities. Another age difference is that at posttest 2, none of the adolescent

animals showed a significant preference for the black compartment, while early

postweanling animals did show a preference for the black compartment. This may be due

to an age-related difference in stress levels. As rats approach adulthood, there is a

55 reduction in the fear response to a stressful stimulus such as the white compartment. This finding also demonstrates that behavioral performance differed based on the apparatus used and the age of the animal.

Experiment 1

Early Postweanling Group

In Experiment 1, results revealed that early postweanling Q-S Group showed a significant preference for the black chamber at both posttests (P 31 and P 40). This result, combined with the results from Experiment 2b, appears to show that D2 primed animals have increased stress levels that may produce a preference for the darker colored compartment. On the other hand, Q-N Group did not show a significant conditioned place aversion at both posttests (P 31 and P 40). This effect is hypothesized to occur because nicotine alleviated the increased anxiety and stress levels in D2 primed animals. This hypothesis is supported by Brioni, O’Neill, Kim, and Decker (1993), who reported that nicotinic receptor agonists such as nicotine and lobeline have anxiolytic effects in rodents behaviorally tested on the elevated plus-maze. They found that mice that were injected either with nicotine or lobeline showed reductions in their stress and fear levels by spending significantly longer in the open arms of the elevated plus-maze. Interestingly, when nicotine-treated mice were injected with a nicotinic receptor antagonist, either mecamylamine or chlorisondamine, the mice exhibited an increased stress and fear response (Brioni et al.). Therefore, it appears that nicotine has anxiolytic properties that are mediated by the nicotinic receptor. In conclusion, the results obtained from the Brioni and colleagues’ study and the current study showed that neonatal quinpirole treatment consistently produced a significant preference for the black compartment, and nicotine

56 has alleviated this preference for the black chamber at both posttests in early

postweanling rats.

An additional finding was that the early postweanling S-N Group visited the white

chamber more often than controls, which means nicotine induced CPP in S-N Group at

posttest 1. This finding is partially consistent with the findings of Brielmaier et al. (2006)

that showed that early postweanling animals (P 28) demonstrated nicotine-induced CPP

when they were given a single acute injection of nicotine in the two-chambered CPP

apparatus. In the current study, nicotine-induced CPP observed at posttest 1 in S-N Group

remained at posttest 2, and, in fact, S-N and Q-N Groups visited the white compartment

equivalently after extended conditioning. Therefore, with further conditioning in the two-

chambered CPP apparatus, nicotine is apparently able to induce CPP or alleviate stress

levels completely in D2 primed early postweanling rats.

Adolescent Group

In the two-chambered CPP study, both D2 and non-D2 primed adolescent rats

administered nicotine (Q-N and S-N Groups) visited the white compartment more than

controls at posttest 1 (P 38). This finding was consistent with Vastola et al. (2004), who

reported that after 12 days of conditioning, nicotine induced CPP in 40-day old

adolescent animals in the two-chambered CPP apparatus. Interestingly, in the current

study, all of the adolescent groups showed a preference for the white chamber at posttest

2 (P 47). In fact, D2 primed adolescent rats actually showed an equivalent preference for the white chamber as controls did. One of the possible reasons for this finding is that all animals were habituated to the white chamber by posttest 2. Also, the D2 primed animals

may have had more of an opportunity to visit the white chamber and were not able to

57 avoid the white chamber easily in the two-chambered CPP apparatus because in the two- chambered CPP apparatus, rats are given only two chambers to choose from. This interesting result was not expected; however, it is assumed that extended training and increased animal age interacted in the two-chambered CPP study to produce the increased preference for the white compartment in adolescent rats at posttest 2. In summary, it appears that in adolescence, neonatal quinpirole treatment does not have a significant effect on CPP performance, at least with the two-chambered paradigm as it was used in this experiment.

Experiment 2a

Early Postweanling Group

In the three-chambered CPP study, results revealed that at posttest 1 (P 31), early postweanling rats behaved in a similar manner as they did in the two-chambered CPP apparatus. However, unlike the results from the two-chambered CPP study, at posttest 2

(P 40), nicotine-induced CPP observed at posttest 1 in Q-N and S-N Groups diminished.

In fact, at posttest 2, all of the animals showed a preference for the black compartment.

This is precisely the opposite effect that was found in adolescent rats using the two- chambered apparatus. Interestingly, these animals began to avoid the white chamber with extended training, whereas adolescent rats began to prefer the white chamber with extended training. As previously mentioned, this curious result was found possibly because animals had more opportunity to avoid the aversive chamber in the three- chambered CPP paradigm. Also, it could be because nicotine-treated animals developed tolerance to nicotine, or the positive reinforcing effects of the drug have diminished.

58 Adolescent Group

Regarding adolescent rats in the three-chambered CPP study, results at posttest 1

(P 38) were similar to the results from the two-chambered CPP study; nicotine induced

CPP, and nicotine appears to have reduced the preference for the black compartment in

D2 primed rats. In contrast, at posttest 2 (P 47), the significant preference for the black compartment observed in Q-S and S-S Groups dissipated, although neither group showed a significant preference for the white compartment, as occurred in Experiment 1 using the two-chambered CPP apparatus. Furthermore, at posttest 2, nicotine-induced CPP observed at posttest 1 in Q-N and S-N Groups diminished. As previously indicated, these findings may be due to increased tolerance to nicotine and habituation to the chamber.

Also, as animals approach adulthood, the aversive properties of the white chamber appear to diminish regardless of drug treatment.

Age Differences in the Two- and Three-Chambered CPP

There were significant age differences in both the two- and three-chambered CPP studies. In each of these studies, the early postweanling S-N Group showed a greater degree of CPP compared to adolescent S-N Group at posttest 1. A possible explanation is that, as previously indicated, younger rats have higher sensitivity to nicotine than older rats (Belluzzi et al., 2004; Brielmaier et al., 2007; Rezvani & Levin, 2004).

Neurochemically, this increased nicotine sensitivity in younger rats may be due to brain development, especially dopamine D2 receptor density development, which takes place during the early postweanling period. D2 receptor density reaches maximum number and density at P 28 and begins to decrease until adulthood (O’Boyle & Waddington, 1984;

Srivastava, Morency, & Mishra, 1991; Tarazi & Baldessarini, 2000). Furthermore, a

59 clinical PET study in smokers has shown that nicotine activates dopamine D2 transmissions in the ventral that is responsible for reward and emotion

(Scott et al., 2007). Therefore, nicotine increases the amount of dopamine in the brain, and dopamine binds to the increased number of D2 receptors in early postweanling rats, which produces positive reinforcement. Thus, early postweanling animals may experience an increased positive reinforcement from nicotine than older animals, so this may help to explain why animals at this age have a tendency to show nicotine-induced

CPP.

Another age difference observed in this study was that at posttest 2, none of the adolescent animals in either the two- or three-chambered CPP studies showed a significant preference for the black compartment, while early postweanling animals did.

In the two-chambered CPP study, D2 priming did not produce a significantly increased preference for the black compartment in adolescent rats at posttest 2, but it did in early postweanling rats at both posttests. In addition, in the three-chambered CPP study at posttest 2, all of the early postweanling animals showed a preference for the black compartment, but none of the adolescent animals showed a preference for either the white or black compartment. These curious results may reflect an age-related change that interacts with the type of behavioral apparatus used. One conclusion to draw would be that D2 primed adolescent animals may demonstrate a less robust increased stress response than their younger counterparts, or they may have less of an aversion to fearful stimuli as they grow older in both the two- and three-chambered CPP apparatuses. It is important to note that nicotine-induced CPP is difficult to achieve in adult rats, and this

60 may be because in part adult controls do not show as strong of an aversion to the white

compartment as adolescents.

Contradicting this claim were the results of the ETM that showed adult D2 primed

animals demonstrated unconditioned fear on the escape trial, although they did not

demonstrate conditioned fear on the baseline and avoidance trials on the ETM. From this

finding, it can be hypothesized that as rats grow older, they experience less conditioned

fear than younger rats. Thus, at posttest 2, both the CPP apparatuses may have measured

only conditioned fear and resulted in adolescent animals not showing a significant

preference for the black compartment.

Also, baseline and avoidance trials of the ETM study can be applied to explain

why all of the adolescent animals showed a preference for the white compartment in the

two-chambered CPP apparatus. When adolescent rats must visit one of the two areas, as

they are forced to do in the two-chambered CPP study, the white chamber may not be as

aversive as when they have more choices, as in the three-chambered CPP apparatus. In

other words, adolescent D2 primed rats visit the white compartment if they have less

opportunity to avoid that stimulus. It is important to note that D2 priming typically

produces an increase in activity, and if D2 primed animals are demonstrating an increase in locomotion, it may essentially be less possible for these animals not to spend a significant amount of time in the white compartment.

The problem with the two-chambered CPP study is that D2 primed adolescent rats

actually showed an equivalent preference for the white chamber as controls in the two-

chambered CPP apparatus, rather than the predicted preference for the black chamber.

The claim has been made that the two-chambered CPP apparatus tests an aversion to one

61 chamber over another due to a lack of alternative choices. However, in this study, it appears that when animals are given chamber choices as in the three-chambered CPP apparatus, animals tend to more easily avoid the white chamber. This may be why none of the adolescent animals showed any preference in the three-chambered CPP.

Experiment 2b

Baseline and Avoidance Trials

The ETM results of Experiment 2b revealed that as opposed to the current study’s hypothesis, there was no significant difference between D2 and non-D2 primed animals on baseline and avoidance trials that measured conditioned fear. This finding suggests that D2 priming does not produce increased conditioned fear.

There was a significant difference observed among female and male rats. Female animals entered one of the open arms significantly more rapidly than male animals on the baseline and avoidance trials. This is possibly because female animals have been reported to have higher activity levels in the open-field test than males, which may induce females to leave the walled area more rapidly than males in the ETM task (Blizard, Lippman, &

Chen, 1975). Consistent with the current ETM study, studies using the elevated plus- maze have reported that male rats did not prefer the open arms as strongly as female rats, and female rats exhibited more exploratory behaviors than males (Johnston & File, 1991;

Lucion, Charchat, Pereira, & Rasia-Filho, 1996). A study by Zimmerberg and Farley

(1993) further investigated these gender differences. They reported both female animals that received chemical castration in the neonatal period and female animals that received ovariectomy in the prepuberty period did not enter the open arms as often as control female rats. On the other hand, gonadectomy in the prepuberty period and chemical

62 castration in the neonatal period did not affect male rats’ preference for the open arms

(Zimmerberg & Farley). Thus, elevated gonadal hormones play an important role in

increased stress and anxiety levels only in females. In contrast, Lucion et al. reported that

to reduce anxiety levels in males, gonads need to be removed during the perinatal period.

In conclusion, it is hypothesized that the current study’s gender differences may have

originated from different periods of elevation of gonadal hormones in both males and

females.

Escape Trial

Results revealed that D2 primed animals took significantly longer to enter the

enclosed arm than non-D2 primed animals on the escape trial that measured

unconditioned fear. It is hypothesized that D2 primed rats took longer because they

exhibited increased freezing behavior on the escape trial, which is rats’ natural response

to high in anxiety and stress. Therefore, this may indicate that D2 primed animals showed

significantly higher anxiety levels than non-D2 primed animals. This hypothesis leads to the conclusion that D2 priming, as produced by neonatal quinpirole treatment, increases

stress and fear levels that may in part account for the overall preference for the black

chamber in D2 primed animals. This finding is consistent with the studies reporting that

stress hormones and dopamine levels are often positively correlated and both are elevated

in schizophrenics (Cabib & Puglisi-Allegra, 1996; Carlsson & Lindqvist, 1963; Cenci et

al., 1992; Finlay & Zigmond, 1997; Yilmaz et al., 2007). Additionally, Rodgers et al.

(1994) reported that D2 receptors play a crucial role in the development of stress and anxiety levels. Therefore, these findings and the present study’s escape trial results support the hypothesis that D2 primed animals have higher stress and fear levels than

63 non-D2 primed animals. In conclusion, D2 primed animals conditioned with saline,

regardless of age, showed a significant conditioned place aversion in both the two- and

three-chambered CPP studies.

Conditioned and Unconditioned Fear in the ETM and CPP

Experiment 2b results revealed that D2 priming produced increased unconditioned fear but not conditioned fear on the ETM trials. This result suggests that D2 primed rats

do not exhibit fear and stress responses if they are conditioned or habituated to the

environment. This hypothesis can be applied to finding observed in both the two- and

three-chambered CPP studies. At posttest 2, in both the two- and three-chambered CPP

apparatuses, early postweanling and adolescent Q-S Group animals and controls were not

significantly different; Q-S Groups did not show a significantly increased preference for

the black compartment compared to controls except in early postweanling Q-S Group in

the two-chambered CPP study. In contrast, at posttest 1, all of the early postweanling and

adolescent Q-S Groups showed a stronger preference for the black compartment

compared to controls in both the two- and three-chambered CPP studies. Therefore, it can

be hypothesized that at posttest 1, the CPP apparatuses measured rats’ unconditioned fear

levels. After 8 days of conditioning, D2 primed rats may not yet have been conditioned or habituated to the environment, so Q-S Groups showed increased unconditioned fear compared to the rest of the groups that resulted in their significant preference for the black compartment. At posttest 2, on the other hand, the CPP apparatuses may have reflected rats’ conditioned fear. After 16 days of conditioning sessions, D2 primed rats

were conditioned to the environment, so D2 primed animals and control groups may have

not been significantly different.

64 Application to the Human Schizophrenic Population

The current CPP and ETM results did not support the hypothesis that D2 primed rats were more likely to develop nicotine-induced CPP than non-D2 primed rats. In fact,

non-D2 primed rats showed a greater degree of nicotine-induced CPP. Also, this study

showed that D2 primed animals experienced increased anxiety and fear levels than non-

D2 primed animals, and their increased anxiety and stress levels appeared to be reduced

by the effects of nicotine. Thus, a conclusion that can be drawn is that the reason why

around 74 % to 92 % of the schizophrenic population in America is nicotine dependent

(de Leon et al., 1995; Hughes et al., 1986; O’Farrell et al., 1983) may be because

schizophrenics smoke cigarettes in order to reduce their anxiety and stress levels. At a

cursory glance, a recommendation for partial treatment for schizophrenics may be to

encourage them to keep smoking cigarettes because their schizophrenic symptoms are

alleviated. However, smoking cigarettes produces multiple consequences, such as lung

cancer, heart and blood vessel diseases, and respiratory diseases (U.S. National Library of

Medicine, 2007a). Especially when smoking habits begins in adolescence, people have a

lesser chance of quitting smoking later in life (DiFranza et al., 2000; Smith et al., 2006).

Thus, smoking cigarettes should not be used to reduce stress and anxiety levels in

adolescent schizophrenics. Currently, there are other ways to consume nicotine such as

nicotine patches and nicotine gum. Although nicotine itself produces side effects such as

high blood pressure, vomiting, difficulty breathing, and agitation (U.S. National Library

of Medicine, 2007b), these methods of ingestion are thought to be less harmful to humans.

Thus, these methods should be encouraged to the schizophrenic population rather than

cigarette smoking. However, it is ideal that future research can lead to other nicotine

65 consumption methods that do not produce addiction to nicotine, or side effects from

nicotine, or prove that other nicotinic receptor agonists can be substituted for nicotine.

Future Studies

Several interesting results were obtained from the current study. One of the most

interesting findings was nicotine induced CPP and appeared to alleviate stress produced

by D2 priming. Based on these findings, several possible future studies can be proposed.

Mecamylamine, a nicotinic receptor antagonist, could be used to block nicotine’s ability

to alleviate stress in D2 primed animals to verify whether nicotinic receptors mediate stress alleviation. Additionally, because D2 priming increases stress levels, a future study

could remove the adrenal glands, which regulate the stress hormone corticosterone, from

the D2 primed animals to verify whether corticosterone mediates the stress response in D2

primed rats. A third possible study could use a D2 receptor antagonist and focus on

whether injection of a D2 receptor antagonist in D2 primed animals reverses the effect of

neonatal quinpirole treatment. Another possible study can focus on neuroanatomy. The

nucleus accumbens shell has been reported to be a mediator of CPP (Spina, Fenu,

Rongoni, Rivas, & Di Chiara, 2006). Therefore, a study could focus on the CPP results

after destruction of dopaminergic neurons in the nucleus accumbens shell by a specific

neurotoxin such as 6-hydroxydopamine to determine how crucial a role the nucleus

accumbens shell plays in the CPP task.

In conclusion, the present study revealed that nicotine has effects on both D2 and

non-D2 primed early postweanling and adolescent rats. Overall, nicotine has been shown

to induce CPP in rats neonatally treated with saline or quinpirole regardless of age. Also,

this study showed that D2 priming produces a significant preference for the black

66 compartment due to increased stress and anxiety levels, but this effect was alleviated by nicotine treatment. Interestingly, there was a significant difference in performance based on the type of apparatus used and animal age, pointing to the importance of behavioral methodology and age when using the CPP paradigm. Furthermore, D2 priming did not appear to drastically elevate the positive reinforcing effects of nicotine compared to saline-treated groups, indicating that D2 priming does not elevate the associative effects of the drug in the CPP apparatus. In addition, this study showed that there is a relationship between nicotine addiction, adolescence, and schizophrenia. Further studies should be conducted related to this matter, which may help us to understand why this relationship exists.

67 CHAPTER 5

REFERENCES

Abi-Dargham, A., Rodenhiser, J., Printz, D., Zea-Ponce, Y., Gil, R., & Kegeles, L. S., et

al. (2000). Increased baseline occupancy of D2 receptors by dopamine in

schizophrenia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United

States of America, 97, 8104-8109.

Abreu-Villaca, Y., Seidler, F. J., Tate, C. A., & Slotkin, T. A. (2003). Nicotine is a

neurotoxin in the adolescent brain: Critical periods, patterns of exposure, regional

selectivity, and dose thresholds for macromolecular alterations. Brain Research,

979, 114-128.

Adler, L. E., Olincy, A., Waldo, M., Harris, J. G., Griffith, J., Stevens K., et al. (1998).

Schizophrenia, sensory gating, and nicotinic receptors. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 24,

189-202.

Adler, L. E., Freedman, R., Ross, R. G., Olincy, A., & Waldo, M. C. (1999). Elementary

phenotypes in the neurobiological and genetic study of schizophrenia. Biological

Psychiatry, 46, 8-18.

Aloe, L., Iannitelli, A., Angelucci, F., Bersani, G., & Fiore, M. (2000). Studies in animal

models and humans suggesting a role of nerve growth factor in schizophrenia-like

disorders. Behavioural Pharmacology, 11, 235-242.

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). DSM-IV, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of

Mental Disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Anzengruber, D., Klump, K. L., Thornton, L., Brandt, H., Crawford, S., Fichter, M. M.,

et al. (2006). Smoking in eating disorders. Eating Behaviors, 7, 291-299.

68 Araki, H., Suemaru, K., & Gomita, Y. (2002). Neuronal nicotinic receptor and

psychiatric disorders: Functional and behavioral effects of nicotine. Japanese

Journal of Pharmacology, 88, 133-138.

Balda, M. A., Anderson, K. L., & Itzhak, Y. (2006). Adolescent and adult responsiveness

to the incentive value of cocaine reward in mice: Role of neuronal nitric oxide

synthase (nNOS) gene. Neuropharmacology, 51, 341-349.

Becker, A., Grecksch, G., Bernstein, H. G., Hollt, V., & Bogerts, B. (1999). Social

behaviour in rats lesioned with ibotenic acid in the hippocampus: Quantitative and

qualitative analysis. Psychopharmacology, 144, 333-338.

Belluzzi, J. D., Lee, A. G., Oliff, H. S., & Leslie, F. M. (2004). Age-dependent effects of

nicotine on locomotor activity and conditioned place preference in rats.

Psychopharmacology, 174, 389-395.

Berger, P. A. (1981). Biochemistry and the schizophrenia. Old concepts and new

hypothesis. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 169, 90-99.

Bernheim, L., Hamann, M., Liu, J. H., Fischer-Lougheed, J., & Bader, C. R. (1996). Role

of nicotinic acetylcholine receptors at the vertebrate myotendinous junction: A

hypothesis. Neuromuscular Disorders: NMD, 6, 211-214.

Bienkowski, P., Kuca, P., Piasecki, J., & Kostowski, W. (1996). Low dose of ethanol

induces conditioned place preference in rats after repeated exposures to ethanol or

saline injections. Alcohol and Alcoholism (Oxford, Oxfordshire), 31, 547-553.

Blizard, D. A., Lippman, H. R., & Chen, J. J. (1975). Sex differences in open-field

behavior in the rat: The inductive and activational role of gonadal hormones.

Physiology & Behavior, 14, 601-608.

69 Breier, A., Buchanan, R. W., Elkashef, A., Munson, R. C., Kirkpatrick, B., & Gellad, F.

(1992). Brain morphology and schizophrenia. A magnetic resonance imaging

study of limbic, prefrontal cortex, and caudate structures. Archives of General

Psychiatry, 49, 921-926.

Breslau, N. (1995). Psychiatric comorbidity of smoking and nicotine dependence.

Behavior Genetics, 25, 95–101.

Brielmaier, J. M., McDonald, C. G., & Smith, R. F. (2007). Immediate and long-term

behavioral effects of a single nicotine injection in adolescent and adult rats.

Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 29, 74-80.

Brioni, J. D., O’Neill, A. B., Kim, D. J., & Decker, M. W. (1993). Nicotinic receptor

agonists exhibit anxiolytic-like effects on the elevated plus-maze test. European

Journal of Pharmacology, 238, 1-8.

Brown, R. W., Gass, J. T., & Kostrzewa, R. M. (2002). Ontogenetic quinpirole treatments

produce spatial memory deficits and enhance skilled reaching in adult rats.

Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 72, 591-600.

Brown, R. W., Flanigan, T. J., Thompson, K. N., Thacker, S. K., Schaefer, T. L., &

Williams, M. T. (2004a). Neonatal quinpirole treatment impairs Morris water task

performance in early postweanling rats: Relationship to increases in

corticosterone and decreases in neurotrophic factors. Biological Psychiatry. 56,

161-168.

Brown, R. W., Thompson, K. D., Thompson, K. N., Ward, J. J., Thacker, S. K, Williams,

M. T., et al. (2004b). Adulthood nicotine treatment alleviates behavioural

impairments in rats neonatally treated with quinpirole: Possible roles of

70 acetylcholine function and neurotrophic factor expression. The European Journal

of Neuroscience, 19, 1634-1642.

Brown, R. W., Perna, M. K., Schaefer, T. L., & Williams, M. T. (2006). The effects of

adulthood nicotine treatment on D2-mediated behavior and neurotrophins of rats

neonatally treated with quinpirole. Synapse (New York, N.Y.), 59, 253-259.

Cabib, S., & Puglisi-Allegra, S. (1996). Stress, depression and the mesolimbic dopamine

system. Psychopharmacology, 128, 331-342.

Cao, Y. J., Surowy, C. S., & Puttfarcken, P. S. (2005). Nicotinic acetylcholine receptor-

mediated [3H]dopamine release from hippocampus. The Journal of

Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 312, 1298-1304.

Carlsson, A., & Lindqvist, M. (1963). Effect of Chlorpromazine or Haloperidol on

Formation of 3Methoxytyramine and Normetanephrine in Mouse Brain. Acta

Pharmacologica et Toxicological, 20, 140-144.

Carr, G. D., Fibiger, H. C., & Phillips, A. G. (1989) Conditioned place preference as a

measure of drug reward. In J. M. Liebman, & S. J. Cooper (Eds.), The

Neuropharmacological Basis of Reward (pp. 264-319). Oxford: Oxford

University Press.

Carew, M. B., & Rudy, J. W. (1991). Multiple functions of context during conditioning:

A developmental analysis. Developmental Psychobiology, 24, 191-209.

Castner, S. A., Vosler, P. S., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (2005). Amphetamine sensitization

impairs cognition and reduces dopamine turnover in primate prefrontal cortex.

Biological Psychiatry, 57, 743–751.

71 Cenci, M. A., Kalen, P., Mandel, R. J., & Bjorklund, A. Regional differences in the

regulation of dopamine and noradrenaline release in medial frontal cortex,

nucleus accumbens and caudate-putamen: A microdialysis study in the rat. Brain

Research, 581, 217-228.

Chambers, R. A., Moore, J., McEvoy, J. P., & Levin, E. D. (1996). Cognitive effects of

neonatal hippocampal lesions in a rat model of schizophrenia.

Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of

Neuropsychopharmacology, 15, 587-594.

Chhatwal, J. P., & Ressler, K. J. (2007). Modulation of fear and anxiety by the

endogenous cannabinoid system. CNS Spectrums, 12, 211-220.

Clarke, P. B., & Reuben, M. (1996). Release of [3H]-noradrenaline from rat hippocampal

synaptosomes by nicotine: Mediation by different nicotinic receptor subtypes

from striatal [3H]-dopamine release. British Journal of Pharmacology, 117, 595-

606.

Cohen, M. (1949). A preliminary report on the ocular findings in 323 schizophrenic

patients. The Psychiatric Quarterly, 23, 667-671.

Crisp, A., Sedgwick, P., Halek, C., Joughin, N., & Humphrey, H. (1999). Why may

teenage girls persist in smoking? Journal of Adolescence, 22, 657-672.

Davis, C. M., Roma, P. G., Dominguez, J. M., & Riley, A. L. (2007). Morphine-induced

place conditioning in Fischer and Lewis rats: Acquisition and dose-response in a

fully biased procedure. Pharmacology, Biochemisry, and Behavior, 86, 516-523.

72 de Leon, J., Dadvand, M., Canuso, C., White, A. O., Stanilla, J. K., & Simpson, G.M.

(1995). Schizophrenia and smoking: An epidemiological survey in a state hospital.

The American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 453-455.

De Marchi, N., De Petrocellis, L., Orlando, P., Daniele, F., Fezza, F., & Di Marzo, V.

(2003). Endocannabinoid signalling in the blood of patients with schizophrenia.

Lipids in Health and Disease, 2, 5.

DiFranza, J. R., Rigotti, N. A., McNeill, A. D., Ockene, J. K., Savageau, J. A., St Cyr, D.,

et al. (2000). Initial symptoms of nicotine dependence in adolescents. Tobacco

Control, 9, 313-319.

D’Souza, D. C., Perry, E., MacDougall, L., Ammerman, Y., Cooper, T., Wu, Y. T., et al.

(2004). The psychotomimetic effects of intravenous delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol

in healthy individuals: Implications for psychosis. Neuropsychopharmacology:

Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 29,

1558-1572.

Durany, N., Michel, T., Zochling, R., Boissl, K. W., Cruz-Sanchez, F. F., Riederer, P., et

al. (2001). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor and neurotrophin 3 in schizophrenic

psychoses. Schizophrenia Research, 52, 79-86.

Fallon, J. H., & Loughlin, S. E. (1995). Substantia nigra. In G. Paxinos (Eds.), The rat

nervous system (pp. 215-237). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Favrat, B., Ménétrey, A., Augsburger, M., Rothuizen, L. E., Appenzeller, M., Buclin, T.,

et al. (2005). Two cases of “cannabis acute psychosis” following the

administration of oral cannabis. BMC Psychiatry, 5:17

73 Ferrari, M. C, Kimura, L., Nita, L. M.., & Elkis, H. (2006). Structural brain abnormalities

in early-onset schizophrenia. Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria, 64, 741-746.

Finlay, J. M., & Zigmond, M. J. (1997). The effects of stress on central dopaminergic

neurons: Possible clinical implications. Neurochemical Research, 22, 1387-1394.

Gage, F. H., Bjorklund, A., Isacson, O., & Brundin, P. (1985). Uses of

neurotransplantation in animal models of neurodegenerative diseases. In G. D.

Das & B. Wallace (Eds.), Neural transplantation and regeneration (pp. 103-124).

New York: Springer-Verlag.

Geyer, M. A., Krebs-Thomson, K., Braff, D. L., & Swerdlow, N. R. (2001).

Pharmacological studies of prepulse inhibition models of sensorimotor gating

deficits in schizophrenia: A decade in review. Psychopharmacology, 156, 117-

154.

Gothelf, D., Soreni, N., Nachman, R. P., Tyano, S., Hiss, Y., Reiner, O., et al. (2000).

Evidence for the involvement of the hippocampus in the pathophysiology of

schizophrenia. European Neuropsychopharmacology: the Journal of the

European College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 10, 389-395.

Gottfried, S. P., & Willner, H. H. (1949). Blood chemistry of schizophrenic patients

before, during and after insulin shock therapy; preliminary studies. Archives of

Neurology and Psychiatry, 62, 809-817.

Graeff, F. G., Netto, C. F., & Zangrossi, H. (1998). The elevated T-maze as an

experimental model of anxiety. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 23,

237-246.

74 Graeff, F. G., Viana, M. B., & Tomaz, C. (1993). The elevated T maze, a new

experimental model of anxiety and memory: Effect of diazepam. Brazilian

Journal of Medical and Biological Research, 26, 67–70.

Haber, S., Barchas, P. R., & Barchas, J. D. (1981). A primate analogue of amphetamine-

induced behaviors in humans. Biological Psychiatry, 16, 181-196.

Harich, S., Gross, G., & Bespalov, A. (2007). Stimulation of the metabotropic glutamate

2/3 receptor attenuates social novelty discrimination deficits induced by neonatal

phencyclidine treatment. Psychopharmacology, 192, 511-519.

Hitzemann, R. (2000). Animal models of psychiatric disorders and their relevance to

alcoholism. Alcohol Research & Health: the Journal of the National Institute on

Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, 24, 149-158.

Holcomb, H. H., Lahti, A. C., Medoff, D. R., Cullen, T., & Tamminga, C. A. (2005).

Effects of noncompetitive NMDA receptor blockade on anterior cingulate

cerebral blood flow in volunteers with schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology:

Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 30,

2275-2282.

Hulshoff Pol, H. E., Schnack, H. G., Bertens, M. G., van Haren, N. E., van der Tweel, I.,

& Staal, W. G., et al. (2002). Volume changes in gray matter in patients with

schizophrenia. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 244-250.

Hughes, J. R., Hatsukami, D. K., Mitchell, J. E., & Dahlgren, L. A. (1986). Prevalence of

smoking among psychiatric outpatients. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 143,

993-997.

75 Isensee, B., Wittchen, H. U., Stein, M. B., Hofler, M., & Lieb, R. (2003). Smoking

increases the risk of panic: Findings from a prospective community study.

Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 692-700.

Janhunen, S., Linnervuo, A., Svensk, M., & Ahtee, L. (2005). Effects of nicotine and

epibatidine on locomotor activity and conditioned place preference in rats.

Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 82, 758-765.

Johnston, A. L., & File, S. E. (1991). Sex differences in animal tests of anxiety.

Physiology & Behavior, 49, 245-250.

Kalsbeek, A., Voorn, P., Buijs, R. M., Pool, C. W., & Uylings, H. B. (1988).

Development of the dopaminergic innervation in the prefrontal cortex of the rat.

The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 269, 58-72.

Kapur, S., Zipursky, R., Jones, C., Remington, G., & Houle, S. (2000). Relationship

between dopamine D(2) occupancy, clinical response, and side effects: A double-

blind PET study of first-episode schizophrenia. The American Journal of

Psychiatry, 157, 514-520.

Klein, L. C., Stine, M. M., Vandenbergh, D. J., Whetzel, C. A., & Kamens, H. M. (2004).

Sex differences in voluntary oral nicotine consumption by adolescent mice: A

dose-response experiment. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 78, 13-25.

Kostrzewa, R. M. (1995). Dopamine receptor supersensitivity. Neuroscience and

Biobehavioral Reviews, 19, 1-17.

Kostrzewa, R. M., Kostrzewa, J. P., Nowak, P., Kostrzewa, R. A., & Brus, R. (2004).

Dopamine D2 agonist priming in intact and dopamine-lesioned rats. Neurotoxicity

Research, 6, 457-462.

76 Krystal, J. H., D’souza, D. C., Gallinat, J., Driesen, N., Abi-Dargham, A., Petrakis, I., et

al. (2006). The vulnerability to alcohol and substance abuse in individuals

diagnosed with schizophrenia. Neurotoxicity Research, 10, 235-252.

Kumari, V., & Postma, P. (2005). Nicotine use in schizophrenia: The self medication

hypotheses. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 29, 1021-34.

Kumari, V., & Sharma, T. (2002). Effects of typical and atypical antipsychotics on

prepulse inhibition in schizophrenia: A critical evaluation of current evidence and

directions for future research. Psychopharmacology, 162, 97-101.

Larrison-Faucher, A. L., Matorin, A. A., & Sereno, A. B. (2004). Nicotine reduces

antisaccade errors in task impaired schizophrenic subjects. Progress in Neuro-

Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 28, 505-516.

Lavalaye, J., Linszen, D. H., Booij, J., Dingemans, P. M., Reneman, L., Habraken, J. B.,

et al. (2001). Dopamine transporter density in young patients with schizophrenia

assessed with [123] FP-CIT SPECT. Schizophrenia Research, 47, 59-67.

Lee, P. R., Brady, D. L., Shapiro, R. A., Dorsa, D. M., & Koenig, J. I. (2005). Social

interaction deficits caused by chronic phencyclidine administration are reversed

by oxytocin. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American

College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 30, 1883-1894.

LeDuc, P. A., & Mittleman, G. (1995). Schizophrenia and psychostimulant abuse: A

review and re-analysis of clinical evidence. Psychopharmacology, 121, 407-427.

Le Foll, B., & Goldberg, S. R. (2005). Nicotine induces conditioned place preferences

over a large range of doses in rats. Psychopharmacology, 178, 481-492.

77 Leonard, S., Gault, J., Hopkins, J., Logel, J., Vianzon, R., Short, M., et al. (2002).

Association of promoter variants in the alpha7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor

subunit gene with an inhibitory deficit found in schizophrenia. Archives of

General Psychiatry, 59, 1085-1096.

Leweke, F. M., Schneider, U., Thies, M., Munte, T. F., & Emrich, H. M. (1999). Effects

of synthetic delta9-tetrahydrocannabinol on binocular depth inversion of natural

and artificial objects in man. Psychopharmacology, 142(3), 230-235.

Leyton, M., Boileau, I., Benkelfat, C., Diksic, M., Baker, G., & Dagher, A. (2002).

Amphetamine-induced increases in extracellular dopamine, drug wanting, and

novelty seeking: A PET/[11C]raclopride study in healthy men.

Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of

Neuropsychopharmacology, 27, 1027-1035.

Liauw, J., Wang, G. D., & Zhuo, M. (2003). NMDA receptors contribute to synaptic

transmission in anterior cingulate cortex of adult mice. Sheng Li Xue Bao: [Acta

Physiologica Sinica], 55, 373-380.

Lipska, B. K., Aultman, J. M., Verma, A., Weinberger, D. R., & Moghaddam, B. (2002).

Neonatal damage of the ventral hippocampus impairs working memory in the rat.

Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of

Neuropsychopharmacology, 27, 47-54.

Lipska, B. K., Jaskiw, G. E., & Weinberger, D. R. (1993). Postpubertal emergence of

hyperresponsiveness to stress and to amphetamine after neonatal excitotoxic

hippocampal damage: A potential animal model of schizophrenia.

78 Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of

Neuropsychopharmacology, 9, 67-75.

Lipton, S. D. (1949). Some comparisons of psychotherapeutic methods in schizophrenia.

Psychiatric Quarterly, 4, 705-714.

Luby, E. D., Cohen, B. D., Rosenbaum, G., Gottlieb, J. S., Kelley, R. (1959). Study of a

new schizophrenomimetic drug; Sernyl. A. M. A. Archives of Neurology and

Psychiatry, 81, 363-369.

Lucion, A. B., Charchat, H., Pereira, G. A., & Rasia-Filho, A. A. (1996). Influence of

early postnatal gonadal hormones on anxiety in adult male rats. Physiology &

Behavior, 60, 1419-1423.

Lyon, E. R. (1999). A review of the effects of nicotine on schizophrenia and

antipsychotic medications. Psychiatric Services (Washington, D.C.), 50, 1346-

1350.

Matsuzawa, S., Suzuki, T., & Misawa, M. (2000). Ethanol, but not the anxiolytic drugs

buspirone and diazepam, produces a conditioned place preference in rats exposed

to conditioned fear stress. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 65, 281-

288.

McKinney, W. T. Jr., & Bunney, W. E. Jr. (1969). Animal model of depression. I.

Review of evidence: Implications for research. Archives of General Psychiatry,

21, 240-248.

Mirnics, K., Middleton, F. A., Stanwood, G. D., Lewis, D. A., & Levitt, P. (2001).

Disease-specific changes in regulator of G-protein signaling 4 (RGS4) expression

in schizophrenia. Molecular Psychiatry, 6, 293-301.

79 Miyasato, K. (2001). Recent advances in research on nicotine dependence and reward

mechanism. Nippon Yakurigaku Zasshi. Folia pharmacologica Japonica, 117, 27-

34.

National Institute of Mental Health. (2006). Schizophrenia. Retrieved September 4, 2006,

from http://www.nimh.nih.gov/healthinformation/schizophreniamenu.cfm

Nabeshima, T., Mouri, A., Murai, R., & Noda, Y. (2006). Animal model of

schizophrenia: Dysfunction of NMDA receptor-signaling in mice following

withdrawal from repeated administration of phencyclidine. Annals of the New

York Academy of Sciences, 1086, 160-168.

Nazarian, A., Russo, S. J., Festa, E. D., Kraish, M., & Quinones-Jenab, V. (2004). The

role of D1 and D2 receptors in the cocaine conditioned place preference of male

and female rats. Brain Research Bulletin, 63, 295-299.

Nerin, I., Beamonte, A., Gargallo, P., Jimenez-Muro, A., & Marqueta, A. (2007). Weight

gain and anxiety levels in recent ex-smokers. Archivos de Bronconeumología, 43,

9-15.

Nowak, P., Brus, R., & Kostrzewa, R. M. (2001). Amphetamine-induced enhancement of

neostriatal in vivo microdialysate dopamine content in rats, quinpirole-primed as

neonates. Polish Journal of Pharmacology, 53, 319-329.

Nugent, T. F. III, Herman, D. H., Ordonez, A., Greenstein, D., Hayashi, K. M., Lenane,

M., et al. (2007). Dynamic mapping of hippocampal development in childhood

onset schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 90, 62-70.

O’Boyle, K. M., & Waddington, J. L. (1984). Loss of rat striatal dopamine receptors with

ageing is selective for D-2 but not D-1 sites: Association with increased non-

80 specific binding of the D-1 ligand [3H]piflutixol. European Journal of

Pharmacology, 105, 171-174.

O’Farrell, T. J., Connors, G. J., & Upper, D. (1983). Addictive behaviors among

hospitalized psychiatric patients. Addictive Behaviors, 8, 329-333.

Oswald, L. M., Wong, D. F., McCaul, M., Zhou, Y., Kuwabara, H., Choi, L., et al. (2005).

Relationships among ventral striatal dopamine release, cortisol secretion, and

subjective responses to amphetamine. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official

Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 30, 821-832.

Parikh, V., Evans, D. R., Kahn, M. M., & Mahadik, S. P. (2003). Nerve growth factor in

never-medicated first-episode psychotic and medicated chronic schizophrenic

patients: Possible implications for treatment outcome. Schizophrenia Research, 60,

117-123.

Parker, L. A. (1992). Place conditioning in a three- or four-choice apparatus: Role of

stimulus novelty in drug-induced place conditioning. Behavioral Neuroscience,

106, 294-306.

Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned . Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Pearce, R. K., Seeman, P., Jellinger, K., & Tourtellotte, W. W. (1990). Dopamine uptake

sites and dopamine receptors in Parkinson’s disease and schizophrenia. European

Neurology, 30 Suppl 1, 9-14.

Perkins, K. A., Donny, E., & Caggiula, A. R. (1999). Sex differences in nicotine effects

and self-administration: Review of human and animal evidence. Nicotine &

Tobacco Research: Official Journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and

Tobacco, 1, 301-315.

81 Piko, B. F., Wills, T. A., & Walker, C. (in press). Motives for smoking and drinking:

Country and gender differences in samples of Hungarian and US high school

students. Addictive Behaviors.

Premkumar, P., Kumari, V., Corr, P. J., & Sharma, T. (2006). Frontal lobe volumes in

schizophrenia: Effects of stage and duration of illness. Journal of Psychiatric

Research, 40, 627-637.

Punnoose, S., & Belgamwar, M. R. (2006). Nicotine for schizophrenia. Cochrane

Database of Systematic Reviews (Online), (1), CD004838.

Raedler, T. J., Knable, M. B., Jones, D. W., Urbina, R. A., Gorey, J. G., Lee, K. S., et al.

(2003). In vivo determination of muscarinic acetylcholine receptor availability in

schizophrenia. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 118-127.

Randrup, A., & Munkvad, I. (1967). Stereotyped activities produced by amphetamine in

several animal species and man. Psychopharmacologia, 11, 300-310.

Rasmussen, B. A., O’Neil, J., Manaye, K. F., Perry, D. C., & Tizabi, Y. (2007). Long-

term effects of developmental PCP administration on sensorimotor gating in male

and female rats. Psychopharmacology, 190, 43-49.

Rezvani, A. H., & Levin, E. D. (2004). Adolescent and adult rats respond differently to

nicotine and alcohol: Motor activity and body temperature. International Journal

of Developmental Neuroscience: the Official Journal of the International Society

for Developmental Neuroscience, 22, 349-354.

Reynolds, L. M., Cochran, S. M., Morris, B. J., Pratt, J. A., & Reynolds, G. P. (2005).

Chronic phencyclidine administration induces schizophrenia-like changes in N-

82 acetylaspartate and N-acetylaspartylglutamate in rat brain. Schizophrenia

Research, 73. 147-152.

Risch, S. C. (1996). Pathophysiology of schizophrenia and the role of newer

antipsychotics. Pharmacotherapy, 16(1 Pt 2), 11-14.

Rodgers, R. J., Nikulina, E. M., & Cole, J. C. (1994). Dopamine D1 and D2 receptor

ligands modulate the behaviour of mice in the elevated plus-maze. Pharmacology,

Biochemistry, and Behavior, 49, 985-995.

Rosenthal, D. (1970). Genetic theory and abnormal behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Russo, S. J., Festa, E. D., Fabian, S. J., Gazi, F. M., Kraish, M., Jenab, S., et al. (2003a).

Gonadal hormones differentially modulate cocaine induced conditioned place

preference in male and female rats. Neuroscience, 120, 523–533.

Russo, S. J., Jenab, S., Fabian, S. J., Festa, E. D., Kemen, L. M., & Quinones-Jenab, V.

(2003b). Sex differences in the conditioned rewarding effects of cocaine. Brain

Research, 970, 214-220.

Salas, R., Pieri, F., Fung, B., Dani, J. A., & De Biasi, M. (2003). Altered anxiety-related

responses in mutant mice lacking the beta4 subunit of the nicotinic receptor. The

Journal of Neuroscience: the Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 23,

6255-6263.

Sams-Dodd, F. (1996). Phencyclidine-induced stereotyped behaviour and social isolation

in rats: A possible animal model of schizophrenia. Behavioural Pharmacology, 7,

3-23.

Sams-Dodd, F. (1998). Effects of continuous D-amphetamine and phencyclidine

administration on social behaviour, stereotyped behaviour, and locomotor activity

83 in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College

of Neuropsychopharmacology, 19, 18-25.

Scheller-Gilkey, G., Thomas, S. M., Woolwine, B. J., & Miller, A. H. (2002). Increased

early life stress and depressive symptoms in patients with comorbid substance

abuse and schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 28, 223-231.

Schenk, S., Ellison, F., Hunt, T., & Amit, Z. (1985). An examination of heroin

conditioning in preferred and nonpreferred environments and in differentially

housed mature and immature rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 22,

215-220.

Scoles, M. T., & Siegel, S. (1986). A potential role of saline trials in morphine-induced

place-preference conditioning. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 25,

1169-1173.

Scott, D. J., Domino, E. F., Heitzeg, M. M., Koeppe, R. A., Ni, L., Guthrie, S., & Zubieta,

J. K. (2007). Smoking modulation of mu-opioid and dopamine D2 receptor-

mediated neurotransmission in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official

Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 32, 450-457.

Seeman, P. (1985). Brain dopamine receptors in schizophrenia and tardive dyskinesia.

Psychopharmacology. Supplementum, 2, 1-8.

Seeman, P. (2006). Targeting the dopamine D2 receptor in schizophrenia. Expert Opinion

on Therapeutic Targets, 10, 515-531.

Seeman P., Corbett R., & Van Tol, H. H. (1997). Atypical neuroleptics have low affinity

for dopamine D2 receptors or are selective for D4 receptors.

84 Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of

Neuropsychopharmacology, 16, 93-110.

Seeman, P., & Lasaga, M. (2005). Dopamine agonist action of phencyclidine. Synapse

(New York, N.Y.), 58, 275-277.

Seeman, P., & Niznik, H. B. (1990). Dopamine receptors and transporters in Parkinson’s

disease and schizophrenia. The FASEB Journal: Official Publication of the

Federation of American Societies for Experimental Biology, 4, 2737-2744.

Seeman, P., Weinshenker, D., Quirion, R., Srivastava, L. K., Bhardwaj, S. K., Grandy, D.

K., et al. (2005). Dopamine supersensitivity correlates with D2High states,

implying many paths to psychosis. Proceedings of the National Academy of

Sciences of the United States of America, 102, 3513-3518.

Sevy, S., Kay, S. R., Opler, L. A., & van Praag, H. M. (1990). Significance of cocaine

history in schizophrenia. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 178, 642-

648.

Shippenberg, T. S., & Heidbreder, C. (1995). Sensitization to the conditioned rewarding

effects of cocaine: Pharmacological and temporal characteristics. The Journal of

Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics, 273, 808-815.

Skosnik, P. D., Krishnan, G. P., Aydt, E. E., Kuhlenshmidt, H. A., & O’Donnell, B. F.

(2006). Psychophysiological evidence of altered neural synchronization in

cannabis use: Relationship to schizotypy. The American Journal of Psychiatry,

163, 1798-1805.

Smith, K. J., Perna, M. K., & Brown, R. W. (n.d.) Neonatal quinpirole treatment

produces prepulse inhibition deficits in adult rats.

85 Smith, L. N., McDonald, C. G., Bergstrom, H. C., Brielmaier, J. M., Eppolito, A. K.,

Wheeler, T. L., et al. (2006). Long-term changes in fear conditioning and anxiety-

like behavior following nicotine exposure in adult versus adolescent rats.

Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior. 85, 91-97.

Soares, J. C., & Innis, R. B. (1999). Neurochemical brain imaging investigations of

schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 46, 600-615.

Solowij, N., Michie, P. T. (2007). Cannabis and cognitive dysfunction: Parallels with

endophenotypes of schizophrenia? Journal of Psychiatry &Nneuroscience: JPN,

32, 30-52.

Spina, L., Fenu, S., Longoni, R., Rivas, E., & Di Chiara, G. (2006). Nicotine-conditioned

single-trial place preference: Selective role of nucleus accumbens shell dopamine

D1 receptors in acquisition. Psychopharmacology, 184, 447-455.

Sprick, U., von Wilmsdorff, M., Bouvier, M. L., Schulz, D., & Gaebel, W. (2006).

Behavioral and hippocampal changes after prenatal invasive interventions with

possible relevance to schizophrenia. Behavioural Brain Research, 172, 179-186.

Srivastava, L. K., Morency, M. A., Bajwa, S. B., & Mishra, R. K. (1990). Effect of

haloperidol on expression of dopamine D2 receptor mRNAs in rat brain. Journal

of Molecular Neuroscience : MN, 2, 155-161.

Stanton, W. R. (1995). DSM-III-R tobacco dependence and quitting during late

adolescence. Addictive Behaviors, 20, 595-603.

Stanton, W. R., McClelland, M., Elwood, C., Ferry, D., & Silva, P. A. (1996). Prevalence,

reliability and bias of adolescents’ reports of smoking and quitting. Addiction

(Abingdon, England), 91, 1705-1714.

86 Tanskanen, P., Veijola, J. M., Piippo, U. K., Haapea, M., Miettunen, J. A., Pyhtinen, J., et

al. (2005). Hippocampus and amygdala volumes in schizophrenia and other

psychoses in the Northern Finland 1966 birth cohort. Schizophrenia Research, 75,

283-294.

Tarazi, F. I., & Baldessarini, R. J. (2000). Comparative postnatal development of

dopamine D(1), D(2) and D(4) receptors in rat forebrain. International Journal of

Developmental Neuroscience: the Official Journal of the International Society for

Developmental Neuroscience, 18, 29-37.

Thacker, S. K., Perna, M. K., Ward, J. J., Schaefer, T. L., Williams, M. T., Kostrzewa, R.

M., et al. (2006). The effects of adulthood olanzapine treatment on cognitive

performance and neurotrophic factor content in male and female rats neonatally

treated with quinpirole. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 2075-2083.

Tollefson, G. D. (1996). Cognitive function in schizophrenic patients. The Journal of

Clinical Psychiatry, 57 Suppl 11, 31-39.

Torkildsen, A. (1949). Notes on the importance of the occipital lobes in a case of

schizophrenia; experience with a case of bilateral occipital leucotomy. Acta

Psychiatrica et Neuroologica, 24, 701-707.

Torrella, T. A., Badanich, K. A., Philpot, R. M., Kirstein, C. L., & Wecker, L. (2004).

Developmental differences in nicotine place conditioning. Annals of the New York

Academy of Sciences. 1021, 399-403.

Toulopoulou, T., Grech, A., Morris, R. A., Schulze, K., McDonald, C., Chapple, B., et al.

(2004). The relationship between volumetric brain changes and cognitive

function: A family study on schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 56, 447-453.

87 Trauth, J. A., Seidler, F. J.,Ali, S. F., & Slotkin, T. A. (2001). Adolescent nicotine

exposure produces immediate and long-term changes in CNS noradrenergic and

dopaminergic function. Brain Research, 892, 269-280.

Trivedi, M. A., & Coover, G. D. (2004). Lesions of the ventral hippocampus, but not the

dorsal hippocampus, impair conditioned fear expression and inhibitory avoidance

on the elevated T-maze. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 81, 172-184.

Tzschentke, T. M. (1998). Measuring reward with the conditioned place preference

paradigm: A comprehensive review of drug effects, recent progress and new

issues. Progress in Neurobiology, 56, 613-672.

Uneri, O., Tural, U., & Cakin Memik, N. (2006). Smoking and schizophrenia: Where is

the biological connection? Turkish Journal of Psychiatry. 17, 55-64.

Ujike, H., & Morita, Y. (2004). New perspectives in the studies on endocannabinoid and

cannabis: Cannabinoid receptors and schizophrenia. Journal of Pharmacological

Sciences, 96, 376-381.

U.S. National Library of Medicine. (2007a). Smoking. Retrieved July 4, 2007, from

http://www.nlm.nih.gov/medlineplus/smoking.html

U.S. National Library of Medicine. (2007b). Medical Encyclopedia. Retrieved July 4,

2007, from http://www.nlm.nih.gov/medlineplus/ency/article/002510.htm

Vastola, B. J., Douglas, L. A., Varlinskaya, E. I. & Spear, L. P. (2002). Nicotine-induced

conditioned place preference in adolescent and adult rats. Physiology & Behavior,

77, 107-114.

Velakoulis, D., Wood, S. J., Wong, M. T., McGorry, P. D., Yung, A., Phillips, L., et al.

(2006). Hippocampal and amygdala volumes according to psychosis stage and

88 diagnosis: A magnetic resonance imaging study of chronic schizophrenia, first-

episode psychosis, and ultra-high-risk individuals. Archives of General Psychiatry,

63, 139-149.

Viana, M. B., Tomaz, C., & Graeff, F. G. (1994). The elevated T-maze: A new animal

model of anxiety and memory. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior, 49,

549-554.

Viveros, M. P., Marco, E. M., & File, S. E. (2006). Nicotine and cannabinoids: Parallels,

contrasts and interactions. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 1161-

1181.

Vizi, E. S., & Kiss, J. P. (1998). Neurochemistry and pharmacology of the major

hippocampal transmitter systems: Synaptic and nonsynaptic interactions.

Hippocampus, 8, 566-607.

Walker, E. F., & Diforio, D. (1997). Schizophrenia: A neural diathesis-stress model.

Psychological Review, 104, 667-685.

Wan, R. Q., Giovanni, A., Kafka, S. H., & Corbett, R. (1996). Neonatal hippocampal

lesions induced hyperresponsiveness to amphetamine: Behavioral and in vivo

microdialysis studies. Behavioural Brain Research, 78, 211-223.

Wang, M. Q., Fitzhugh, E. C., Green, B. L., Turner, L. W., Eddy, J. M.,& Westerfield,

R.C. (1999). Prospective social-psychological factors of adolescent smoking

progression. The Journal of Adolescent Health: Official Publication of the Society

for Adolescent Medicine, 24, 2-9.

89 Wass, C., Archer, T., Palsson, E., Fejgin, K., Alexandersson, A., Klamer, D., et al. (2006).

Phencyclidine affects memory in a nitric oxide-dependent manner: Working and

reference memory. Behavioural Brain Research, 174, 49-55.

Wilkie, G. I., Hutson, P., Sullivan, J. P., & Wonnacott, S. (1996). Pharmacological

characterization of a nicotinic autoreceptor in rat hippocampal synaptosomes.

Neurochemical Research, 21, 1141-1148.

Wilson, J. F. (2003). Biological foundations of human behavior. Belmont, CA:

Wadsworth/Thomson Learning.

Winklbaur, B., Ebner, N., Sachs, G., Thau, K., & Fischer, G. (2006). Substance abuse in

patients with schizophrenia. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 8, 37-43.

Wood, G. K., Lipska, B. K., & Weinberger, D. R. (1997). Behavioral changes in rats with

early ventral hippocampal damage vary with age at damage. Brain Research.

Developmental Brain Research, 101, 17-25.

Woodruff, M. L., & Baisden, R. H. (1994) Trimethyltin neurotoxicity in rat as an

anoalogous model of Alzheimer’s disease. In M. L. Woodruff & A. J. Nonneman

(Eds.), Toxin-induced models of neurological disorders (pp. 319-335). New York:

Plenum Press.

Xu, Z., Seidler, F. J., Ali, S. F., Slikker, W. Jr., & Slotkin, T. A. (2001). Fetal and

adolescent nicotine administration: effects on CNS serotonergic systems. Brain

Research, 914, 166-178.

Xu, Z., Seidler, F. J., Cousins, M. M., Slikker, W. Jr., Slotkin TA. (2002). Adolescent

nicotine administration alters serotonin receptors and cell signaling mediated

through adenylyl cyclase. Brain Research, 951, 280-292.

90 Yamada, M., Hirao, K., Namiki, C., Hanakawa, T., Fukuyama, H., Hayashi, T., et al.

(2007). Social cognition and frontal lobe pathology in schizophrenia: A voxel-

based morphometric study. Neuroimage, 35, 292-298.

Yamasue, H., Iwanami, A., Hirayasu, Y., Yamada, H., Abe, O., Kuroki, N., et al. (2004).

Localized volume reduction in prefrontal, temporolimbic, and paralimbic regions

in schizophrenia: An MRI parcellation study. Psychiatry Research, 131, 195-207.

Yang, X., Criswell, H. E., & Breese, G. R. (1996). Nicotine-induced inhibition in medial

septum involves activation of presynaptic nicotinic cholinergic receptors on

gamma-aminobutyric acid-containing neurons. The Journal of Pharmacology and

Experimental Therapeutics, 276, 482-489.

Yilmaz, N., Herken, H., Cicek, H. K., Celik, A., Yurekli, M., & Akyol, O. (2007).

Increased levels of nitric oxide, cortisol and adrenomedullin in patients with

chronic schizophrenia. Medical principles and practice: international journal of

the Kuwait University, Health Science Centre, 16, 137-141.

Zangrossi, H. Jr., & Graeff, F. G. (1997). Behavioral validation of the elevated T-maze, a

new animal model of anxiety. Brain Research Bulletin, 44, 1-5.

Zhang, X. Y., Tan, Y. L., Zhou, D. F., Haile, C. N., Wu, G. Y., Cao, L. Y., et al. (in

press). Nicotine dependence, symptoms and oxidative stress in male patients with

schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American

College of Neuropsychopharmacology.

Zimmerberg, B., & Farley, M. J. (1993). Sex differences in anxiety behavior in rats: role

of gonadal hormones. Physiology & Behavior, 54, 1119-1124.

91 VITA

YOKO E. OGAWA

Personal Data: Date of Birth: April 22, 1982

Place of Birth: Osaka, Japan

Marital Status: Single

Education: Nishinomiya Kita High School, Hyogo, Japan

Leysin American School, Leysin, Switzerland

B.S. Psychology, Wittenberg University, Springfield, Ohio 2005

M.A. Psychology, East Tennessee State University, Johnson City, Tennessee 2007

Professional Experience: Research Assistant, Wittenberg University, Department of Psychology, May 2004-July2004, August 2004-December 2004.

Research Assistant, Quillen College of Medicine, East Tennessee State University, The Office of Continuing Medical Education, Johnson City, Tennessee, May 2006-Augusut 2006.

Graduate Assistant, East Tennessee State University, Department of Psychology, Johnson City, Tennessee, August 2005-May 2007

Publications: Wilson, J. F., Sippola, S. A., Ogawa, Y. E., Mehicic, M. A., Beaverson, T. M., & Sandoval, M. D. (2007). Effects of sugar on during lunchtime meals. In revision for Publication in Appetite.

Pederson, C. L., Wilson, J. F., Austen, L., Beaulieu, J., Ogawa, Y. E., Kubelka, L. M., & Vickers, C. M. (2007). Adverse childhood experiences and adult body mass index in women. In revision for publication in Child Abuse & Neglect.

Yin, D., Woodruff, M. L., Ogawa, Y. E., & Brown, R. W. (2007). The effects of chronic stress on

92 cognition and genetic expression in the hippocampus. Manuscript in preparation.

Ogawa, Y. E., Cooper, E. L., Bruner, C. L., Baisden, R., & Brown, R. W. (2007). The effects of nicotine conditioned place preference in D2-primed adolescent rats: Age-related and gender effects. Manuscript in preparation.

Presentations: Jennifer A., Ogawa, Y. E., Cooper, E. L., Bruner, C. L., & Brown, R. W. (2007, March). The effects of nicotine conditioned place preference in D2-primed adolescent rats: Age-related and gender effects. Poster session presented at the Appalachian Student Research Forum, Johnson City, TN.

Ogawa, Y. E., Cooper, E. L., Bruner, C. L., Perna, M. K., Thompson, K. N., Baisden, R., & Brown, R. W. (2006, October). The effects of nicotine conditioned place preference in D2-primed adolescent rats: Age-related and gender effects. Poster session presented at the Society for Neuroscience Annual Meeting, Atlanta, GA.

Maple, A. M., Perna, M. K, Ogawa, Y. E., Longacre, I. D., Woodruff, M. L., & Brown, R. W. (2006, June). Nicotine alleviation of deficits in prepulse inhibition in a rodent model of schizophrenia are blocked by mecamylamine. Poster session presented at the College on Problems of Drug Dependence Annual Meeting, Scottsdale, AZ.

Cooper, E. L., Ogawa, Y. E., Bruner, C. L., Perna, M. K., Thompson, K. N., Baisden, R., & Brown, R. W. (2006, March). The effects of nicotine conditioned place preference in D2-primed adolescent rats: Age-related and gender effects. Poster session presented at the Appalachian Student Research Forum, Johnson City, TN.

Ogawa, Y. E., Brown, R. W., Woodruff, M. L., & Yin, D. (2006, March). Cognitive deficits and genetic alterations produced by chronic stress in mice. Poster session presented at the Appalachian Student Research Forum, Johnson City, TN.

93 Correll, J. A., Thompson, K. N., Ogawa, Y. E., Longacre, I. D., Yin, D., Woodruff, M. L., & Brown, R. W. (2006, March). Nicotine sensitization in adolescent -arrestin 2 knockout mice. Poster session presented at the Appalachian Student Research Forum, Johnson City, TN.

Wilson, J. F., Ogawa, Y. E., & Engle, K. (2005, July). Increased blood sugar levels associated with food variety. Poster session presented at the Annual Meeting of the Society for the Study of Ingestive Behavior, Pittsburgh, PA.

94